Conceptualization based on the interpretation of textual information. Conceptualization and categorization as a means of organizing the information space. Recommended list of dissertations

At this stage, a meaningful analysis of the problem area is carried out, the concepts used and their relationships are identified, methods for solving problems are determined. This stage ends with the creation of a domain model (SW), which includes the main concepts and relationships. At the stage of conceptualization, the following features of the problem are determined:

  • types of available data;
  • initial and output data, subtasks of the general task;
  • applied strategies and hypotheses;
  • types of relationships between software objects, types of relationships used (hierarchy, cause - effect, part - whole, etc.);
  • the processes applied during the decision;
  • composition of knowledge used in solving the problem;
  • types of restrictions imposed on the processes that are applied during the solution;
  • composition of knowledge used to justify decisions.

There are two approaches to the process of building a domain model, which is the goal of ES developers at the conceptualization stage. Indicative or Attributive Approach implies the presence of information received from experts in the form of triples object-attribute-attribute value, as well as the presence of training information. This approach is being developed within the framework of a direction called "knowledge formation" or "machine learning" (machine learning).

The second approach, called structural (or cognitive), is carried out by highlighting the elements of the subject area, their relationships and semantic relations.

The attributive approach is characterized by the presence of the most complete information about the subject area: about objects, their attributes, and attribute values. In addition, an essential point is the use of additional training information, which is specified by grouping objects into classes according to one or another content criterion. Object-attribute-attribute value triplets can be obtained using the so-called reclassification method, which is based on the assumption that the task is object-oriented and the task objects are well known to the expert. The idea of ​​the method is that rules (combinations of attribute values) are constructed to distinguish one object from another. The learning information can be given on the basis of precedents of correct expert opinions, for example, using a knowledge extraction method called "thought-aloud protocol analysis".

In the presence of training information for the formation of a model of the subject area at the stage of conceptualization, you can use the entire arsenal of methods developed within the framework of the problem of pattern recognition. Thus, despite the fact that there is not much space given to the attributive approach here, it is one of the consumers of everything that was indicated in the lecture on pattern recognition and automatic data grouping.

A structural approach to building a domain model involves the allocation of the following cognitive elements of knowledge:

1. Concepts.

2. Relationships.

3. Metaconcepts.

4. Semantic relations.

The selected concepts of the subject area should form a system, which is understood as a set of concepts that has the following properties:

  • uniqueness (lack of redundancy);
  • completeness (a fairly complete description of various processes, facts, phenomena, etc. of the subject area);
  • reliability (validity - the correspondence of the selected units of semantic information to their real names) and consistency (lack of homonymy).

When constructing a system of concepts using "local view method" the expert is asked to break the task into subtasks to list the target states and describe the general categories of the target. Further, for each partition (local representation), the expert formulates informational facts and gives them a clear name (name). It is believed that in order to successfully solve the problem of constructing a domain model, the number of such informational facts in each local representation that a person is able to manipulate simultaneously should be approximately equal to seven.

"Method of calculating the utilization factor" based on the following hypothesis. A data element (or information fact) may be a concept if it:

  • used with a large number of other data elements;
  • rarely used in conjunction with other data items compared to total number its application in all subtasks (this is the utilization factor).

The obtained values ​​can serve as a criterion for the classification of all data elements and, thus, for the formation of a system of concepts.

"Method of forming a list of concepts" consists in the fact that the experts (preferably more than two) are given the task of compiling a list of concepts related to the subject area under study. The concepts identified by all experts are included in the system of concepts, the rest are subject to discussion.

"Role Method" consists in the fact that the expert is given the task to train the knowledge engineer to solve some problems of the subject area. Thus, the expert plays the role of a teacher, and the knowledge engineer plays the role of a student. The learning process is recorded on a tape recorder. Then the third participant listens to the tape and writes down on paper all the concepts used by the teacher or student.

When using the method "compiling a list of elementary actions" the expert is given the task to compile such a list when solving the problem in an arbitrary order.

In the method "compilation of the table of contents of the textbook" the expert is asked to imagine a situation in which he was asked to write a textbook. It is necessary to draw up on paper a list of proposed chapters, sections, paragraphs, paragraphs and subparagraphs of the book.

"Textological method" formation of a system of concepts lies in the fact that the expert is given the task to write out from the manuals (books in the specialty) some elements that are units of semantic information.

The group of methods for establishing relationships involves the establishment of semantic proximity between individual concepts. The establishment of relationships is based on the psychological effect of "free associations", as well as the fundamental category of proximity of objects or concepts.

The effect of free associations is as follows. The subject is asked to respond to a given word with the first word that comes to mind. As a rule, the reaction of most of the subjects (if the words were not too unusual) turns out to be the same. The number of hops in a chain can serve as a measure of the "semantic distance" between two concepts. Numerous experiments confirm the hypothesis that for any two words (concepts) there is an associative chain consisting of no more than seven words.

"Method of free associations" based on the psychological effect described above. The expert is presented with a concept with a request to name the first concept that came to mind from the previously formed system of concepts as soon as possible. Next, the information received is analyzed.

In the "card sorting" method The concepts written on the cards serve as the source material. Two versions of the method are used. In the first one, the expert is given some global criteria of the subject area, which he should be guided by when laying out the cards into groups. In the second case, when it is impossible to formulate global criteria, the expert is given the task to decompose the cards into groups in accordance with an intuitive understanding of the semantic similarity of the presented concepts.

"Regularity Detection Method" is based on the hypothesis that the elements of the concept chain, which a person remembers with a certain regularity, have a close associative relationship. For the experiment, 20 concepts are randomly selected. The expert is presented with one of the selected ones. The procedure is repeated up to 20 times, and each time the initial concepts must be different. Then the knowledge engineer analyzes the received chains in order to find constantly repeating concepts (regularities). Associative relationships are established within the groupings identified in this way.

In addition to the informal methods discussed above, formal methods are also used to establish relationships between individual concepts. These primarily include methods semantic differential and repertory grids.

The selected concepts of the subject area and the relationships established between them serve as the basis for further construction of a system of metaconcepts - a system of concept groupings meaningful in the context of the subject area under study. To determine these groupings, both informal and formal methods are used.

Interpretation, as a rule, is easier for an expert if the groupings are obtained by informal methods. In this case, the selected classes are more understandable to the expert. Moreover, in some subject areas it is not at all necessary to establish relationships between concepts, since meta-concepts, figuratively speaking, "lie on the surface".

The last step in building a domain model in conceptual analysis is the establishment of semantic relationships between the selected concepts and metaconcepts. To establish semantic relationships means to determine the specifics of the relationship obtained as a result of the application of certain methods. To do this, it is necessary to comprehend each recorded relationship and attribute it to one or another type of relationship.

There are about 200 basic relations, for example, "part - whole", "genus - species", "cause - effect", spatial, temporal and other relations. For each subject area, in addition to common basic relationships, there may be unique relationships.

"Direct method" establishing semantic relationships is based on a direct understanding of each relationship. In the case when the expert finds it difficult to interpret the highlighted relationship, he is offered the following procedure. Triples are formed: concept 1 - connection - concept 2. Next to each triple, a short sentence or phrase is written, constructed so that concept 1 and concept 2 are included in this sentence. Only meaningful relationships are used as connectives, and indefinite connectives such as "similar to" or "related to" are not used.

For "indirect method" it is not necessary to have relationships, it is enough just to have a system of concepts. A certain criterion is formulated, for which a certain set of concepts is selected from the system of concepts. This set is presented to the expert with a request to give a verbal description of the formulated criterion. Concepts are presented to the expert all at once (preferably on cards). In case of difficulties, the expert resorts to dividing the selected concepts into groups using smaller criteria. The initial number of concepts can be arbitrary, but after splitting into groups, each of such groups should contain no more than ten concepts. After the descriptions for all groups are compiled, the expert is offered to combine these descriptions into one.

The next step in the indirect method of establishing semantic relationships is the analysis of a text composed by an expert. Concepts are replaced with numbers (this may be the original numbering), and the links are left. Thus, a certain graph is constructed, the vertices of which are concepts, and the arcs are bundles (for example, "in view of", "leads to", "expressed from one side", "conditioning", "combining", "defines", "up to" etc.) This method allows you to establish not only basic relationships, but also relationships specific to a particular subject area.

The above methods of forming a system of concepts and metaconcepts, establishing relationships and semantic relations in various combinations are used at the stage of conceptualization when building a model of the subject area.

Further analysis is aimed at comparing and summarizing the described data in order to "build" them into a certain concept, a "mini-theory" of the described events.

Primary classification of data Classification, or nomination, of data - ϶ᴛᴏ combining the content of textual information into a certain generalized category - into one class. It is worth noting that it is extremely important for comparing different information (evidence) received from one respondent within one "case" or for comparing the ϶ᴛᴏth "case" with similar ones. So, statements about parents, relatives can be combined into the class "attitude to the family."

Some data can be classified ("nominated") at once. For example, in our excerpt from the biography of the "Arbat resident" P., we record his gender and place of birth. Other information requires reflection from the point of view scientific concepts. For example, the above passage can be broadly classified under the concept of "identity"; or more specifically in terms of "identification with small homeland"; "identification with the liberal intelligentsia". It is this primary nomination that depends on the goals of the study and requires further clarification (narrowing or, conversely, expanding) after comparison with other episodes of one biography or interviews with other people. The concept of "identity" is appropriate if we we are going to interpret the problem in theories of personal "we-identification". At the same time, the same fragment can be classified in terms of "primary cultural environment", or rather, "cultural environment of the Arbat", if the problem of changes in cultural orientations throughout the entire life path person.

Here it is appropriate to recall once again that the direction of analysis depends not only on the content of the text, but, of course, also on the research interest of the sociologist, his attitude that problematizes the event. In fact, in the excerpt from the interview with P., there are both problems of social identification and the problem of life perception of the cultural environment as the environment itself matures and socio-historical changes. The sociologist is more likely to choose to conceptualize in terms of cultural environment, while the social psychologist is likely to prefer to classify narrative fragments in terms of personal identification.

The logic of the analysis may lead to the need to introduce additional nominations that were not previously provided or have no basis in this passage. Generalization of nominations is a method by which particular data are combined into a certain set of similar phenomena, which can be expressed in a system of sociological concepts.

Note that the theoretical understanding of the data is a very delicate and time-consuming analytical work. In the process of classification, one has to repeatedly refer to the primary text again and again in order to more accurately formulate a sociological concept that is most suitable for all fragments, passages and all analyzed texts. It is this analytical-intuitive activity of the researcher that requires imagination, understanding of the text and the social reality behind it, but above all, a sufficient sociological culture.18

18 The famous logician Mario Bunge said that intuition is ϶ᴛᴏ "trash in the attic of our memory", from which, if necessary, we extract the necessary items.

There is an interaction between the problem-theoretical "field" of the researcher and a fragment of social reality represented by the text, in the process of which different categories are "tried on" and the final choice of the paradigm most suitable for this study occurs. The solution does not have to be mechanically neutral; it comes from certain research goals - what will I achieve as a result of using certain classification concepts?

Let us study an example of the classification of statements about the role and authority of the father in the family from the study by E. Meshcherkina [172. p. 312]; "... Conflicts with my stepfather? No, there were, of course, sometimes. But, by the way, he never liked to teach me anything or read morality there. Mostly mother, of course. It is worth noting that she even forced him with a strap whip me, but ϶ᴛᴏ was rare. I grew up, got stronger ... Besides, I was then two years old, so I was in 9-10 grade, I was doing karate, I knew how to do something. I even remember a couple of times - I beat him. And since then he hasn’t bothered me at all Well, so, yelling ... "

Classification:

* relationship with father (use of force in education, "hard" practice)

* the role of the stepfather and mother in education (the peripheral role of the stepfather, the active role of the mother)

* change in family relations as the son grows up (playing sports, realizing aggressiveness, dominance)

* son's response to his stepfather (reciprocal use of force in a dispute with him)

Later, according to the classifications put forward here, it is possible to compare the system of relations between father and son with other cases where similar information is presented. It is possible that some of the categories will be clarified, others enlarged, additional categories will appear, but it is the primary classifications of the ϶ᴛᴏ interview that become the basis for a comparative analysis of the father-son relationship in other texts.

Classifications should not overlap, they resemble an unordered nominal scale in a quantitative approach: the described fragments are either "included" or "excluded" in this nomination. Subsequently, causal links (dependency links) between the classified fragments can be revealed only if this requirement is met.

Classification rules:

Classification will be the first step of the conceptual analysis.

It provides for a break in the continuous text and its new construction, now on the basis of generalized concepts.

Classification is carried out on the basis of common sense combined with the researcher's analytical intuition.

the main objective classification - comparison of fragmentary data, their generalization.

Multiple revision of the selected categories contributes to a more adequate nomination.

The final conceptualization should correspond to the problematic goals of the study.

It is the classification that underlies the identification of dependencies. Note that now that everyday concepts are classified, we can identify patterns, variations and deviations in the data, discover common meanings and communication patterns within the same community.

CHAPTER I. THEORETICAL FOUNDATIONS OF SCIENTIFIC

TRANSLATION.

1.1. Characteristics of the scientific translation strategy.

1.1.1. The concept of translation strategy.

1.1.2. Factors of scientific translation strategy.

1.1.2.1. Objective factors of scientific translation strategy.

1.1.2.2. Subjective factors of scientific translation strategy.

1.2. The specifics of the scientific text.

1.2.1. Conceptualization as the basis of a scientific text

1.2.2. The concept of scientific concept.

1.2.3. Concepts of mental model and schema.

1.2.4. The role of the term and definition in the scientific text.

1.2.5. The operation of the principle of systematization in a scientific text.

1.2.6. Logical relations between the basic units of scientific text as the basis of conceptualization.

1.2.7. Intratextuality of the scientific text.

1.2.8. Intertextuality of scientific text.

1.3. Peculiarities of scientific text translation.

1.3.1. The concept of scientific translation in the aspect of cognitive-activity translation studies.

1.3.2. Semiotic aspect of scientific text translation.

1.3.3. Cognitive aspect of scientific text translation.

1.3.4. The communicative aspect of scientific text translation.

Conclusions to chapter I.

CHAPTER I. RESEARCH OF CONCEPTUALIZATION IN SCIENTIFIC TRANSLATION.

2.1. Methodology for the study of conceptualization in scientific translation.

2.2. Comparative analysis of the monograph D.Crystal ^ "English as a global language" and its translation into Russian (monograph i).

2.3. Comparative analysis of the monograph by N.Chomsky "Language and problems of knowledge" and its translation into Russian (monograph 2).

2.4. Comparative analysis of scientific articles and their translations. Typology of translation errors.

Conclusions to chapter II.

Recommended list of dissertations

  • Linguistic and cognitive aspects of the translation of the poem by N.V. Gogol "Dead Souls" in English 2010, candidate of philological sciences Moroz, Narkiza Abrikovna

  • Typological dominant of the text in the theory of poetic translation 2012, Doctor of Philology Shutyomova, Natalia Valerievna

  • Asymmetry of the Concept in the Light of the Cognitive-Activity Approach in Translation Studies: Based on the Material of M.A. Bulgakov "The Master and Margarita" and its translations into English 2010, candidate of philological sciences Dzida, Natalya Nikolaevna

  • Correlation between the original and the translation of a literary text: an isomorphic-cognitive approach 2009, candidate of philological sciences Sokolovsky, Yaroslav Viktorovich

  • Features of translation of cognitive spaces in the speaker's knowledge structure 2006, candidate of philological sciences Mordovskaya, Elena Vyacheslavovna

Introduction to the thesis (part of the abstract) on the topic "Conceptualization as a strategy for the translation of a scientific text: Based on the Russian and English languages"

Currently, translation studies is one of the actively developing areas of modern linguistics, characterized by anthropocentrism (the study of objects according to their role, purpose and functions for a person), expansionism (a tendency towards integration processes, which result in interdisciplinary research), neofunctionalism (integration of scientific disciplines to build theory of language use) and explanatory (the desire to explain phenomena from different points of view) (Kubryakova 1995).

The relevance of the study of scientific translation, one of the types of translation activities, is due to both linguistic and extralinguistic factors. Linguistic justification can be considered the need for further development of the theory of scientific translation, as well as the need to improve its methodology. An analysis of the problems of scientific translation reveals the lack of sufficient knowledge about the methods of translation of a special text. The study of the translation strategy of a scientific text will reveal its specifics and thereby optimize the translation process.

Extralinguistic factors include the progress of science, which entails the growth scientific publications requiring high-quality translation. Therefore, the more successfully the translation of a scientific text is carried out, the faster its objectification occurs in the foreign language community.

In modern translation studies, two trends are expressed in the study of the translation process, based on the type of text and the type of cognition:

1) the study of the translation of a scientific text in the aspect of traditional translation studies of a substitutive-transformational nature

Borisova 2001; Gorodetsky 1994; Dovbysh 2003; Evteev 2003; Emuzova 2004).

2) the study of the translation of a literary text in the aspect of modern cognitive-activity translation studies (Klyukanov 1999; Ruschakov 1997; Sorokin 1991; Sorokin 2003; Sorokin 2004; Fesenko 2002; Khairullin 1995).

The problems of theory and practice of literary translation are among those actively solved. This is explained by artistic creativity was the subject of research various sciences over the centuries, beginning with the writings of Aristotle. Thereby literary translation has rich theoretical and practical material.

Scientific translation, having taken shape in the middle of the 20th century, is a relatively “young” field of translation studies. Research in this direction is associated with tasks solved by terminology and stylistics - the sciences that study the scientific text and scientific creativity. Linguistic disciplines of the middle of the XX century. developed under the influence of the then dominant paradigm of knowledge-structuralism.

Within the framework of traditional translation studies (Barkhudarov 1975; Kade 1978; Komissarov 1973; Minyar-Beloruchev 1980; Naida 1978; Newmark 1988; Retsker 1974; Fedorov 1983; Schweitzer 1988;) two types of translation were studied: artistic and informative. The literary included the translation of works of fiction, the informative - the translation of scientific, technical and official business texts (Komissarov 1973). The problems of translating scientific and technical texts were mainly reduced to the difficulties of translating individual terms, to the transfer of their grammatical, lexical and syntactic features. The terms were interpreted as closed nomenclature units of a certain branch of science or technology. The difficulty in translating the term was to find a foreign equivalent (Minyar-Beloruchev 1980), which has the same stylistic coloring and genre as the original term (Fedorov 1983).

In modern translation studies, the study of scientific translation, delimited from technical, has not become widespread. The problems of translation of scientific and technical types of text are studied within the framework of a single scientific and technical direction, where translation is interpreted as a “terminological translation activity” (Gorodetsky 1994) or as a “process of choosing a translation equivalent” (Borisova 2000).

The nature of the technical and scientific text types and, accordingly, the types of translations are different (Alekseeva 2002i; Raie 1978; Torop 1982). So, K. Rice believes that the typology of texts that meets the requirements of translation is a prerequisite for an objective assessment of translations, since the original type of text determines the method and means of translation (Rais 1978). JI.M. Alekseeva believes that there are as many translation techniques as there are types of text and types of communication, since different types of text are understood by the translator in different ways (Alekseeva 2002i).

In the present study, the interpretation of the translation of a scientific text as a special type of translation is based on the general theories of language set forth in the works of W. von Humboldt (Humboldt 1984), Yu.M. Lotman (Lotman 1972; Lotman 1984), C.W. Morris (Morris 1998i), C.S. Pierce (Pierce 1998), A.A. Potebnya (Potebnya 1976; Potebnya 1990; Potebnya 1993), E. Sepira (Sepir 1993), G.G. Shpet (Shpet 2003), R. Jacobson (Jacobson 1985i). They determine the methodology of the dissertation research, allow us to consider the translation strategy of a scientific text in relation to the type of text and type of communication.

Problems of the scientific text are solved by L.M. Alekseeva (Alekseeva 1999, Alekseeva 20021, Alekseeva 2002, Alekseeva

2002h, Alekseeva 20 024, Alekseeva 2004i, Alekseeva 2004). In these works, the features of a scientific text that affect its understanding by a translator, and ways of translating special knowledge into a translated text are studied. The concept of scientific translation developed by L.M. Alekseeva, as well as the work of domestic and foreign researchers in the field of translation theory and cognitive science, served as the theoretical basis for the dissertation work.

The ongoing research is based on such concepts as the type of text, type of communication, thinking, cognition, understanding, personality, individual creativity, cognitive model, concept, etc., which determine the complex phenomenon of scientific translation. The translation of a scientific text is interpreted by us simultaneously as a linguistic and cognitive activity of a person, which is under the influence of various factors.

One of the main problems of translation of a scientific text is the method of translation of the original scientific knowledge, which determines the strategy for the translation of a scientific text and, as a result, affects the quality of the translated text. The concept of the translation strategy of a scientific text is based on the ideas of V.Z. Demyankov (Demyankov 20011), R. Karnapa (Sagpar 1992), I.E. Klyukanov (Klyukanov 1999), A.N. Kryukova (Kryukov 1989), E.S. Kubryakova (Kubryakova 2004), J. Lakoff (Lakoff 1988; Lakoff 1995), M.V. Nikitin (Nikitin 2003), M. Turner, J. Fauconnier (Turner, Fauconnier 1995; Turner, Fauconnier 2001), R. Schenk (Shank 1988) and others, related to the fact that this concept is due to the nature of the scientific type of text.

In the scientific type of text, two principles interact: rational-logical and figurative (Zheltukhina 2003). The first substantiates the solution of the problem of establishing relations between the components of the text and checking the mandatory truth of these relations, i.e. their verifiability.

The second is related to modeling, a freer activity associated with the search for rules, conditions and design possibilities.

The study of the translation of a scientific text based on these principles seems to be promising, since the translation of a scientific text is understood by us as a complex process involving linguistic, cognitive, communicative mechanisms that allow us to accurately recreate the original scientific knowledge in the translated text.

The purpose of the study is to identify the specifics of conceptualization in scientific translation and the possibility of considering it as a strategy for the translation of a scientific text. The study of the process of conceptualization in a scientific text is associated with the desire to confirm the fact that the translation of a scientific text is conditioned by the type of text and the type of communication.

The goal is to solve the following tasks:

Analyze the conditions and factors for choosing a scientific translation strategy;

Consider the reasons for choosing conceptualization as a strategy for the translation of a scientific text;

Determine the language means of expressing a scientific concept;

Conduct a comparative analysis of the originals and translations of a scientific text in order to identify cases of translation failures, as well as describe and classify translation errors;

Establish correspondence/discrepancy between the logical relationships underlying the conceptualization of the source and translated texts;

Show the effectiveness of choosing a conceptualization based on the types of logical relations as a possible strategy for the translation of a scientific text.

The object of the dissertation research is the linguistic ways of expressing a scientific concept in the original (IT) and translated texts (PT).

The subject of the study is the types of logical relationships between the key words expressing the scientific concept in the original and translated texts.

The study is based on the following hypothesis: the process of conceptualization in the translated text is similar to the process of conceptualization in the source text, built on a certain type of logical relations.

This assumption allows us to consider conceptualization as a factor in the translation strategy of a scientific text. Due to the fact that there is no direct way to detect concepts in a scientific text, we, following J. Lakoff and J. Fauconnier, consider it possible to apply the modeling method to detect them.

The scientific novelty of the ongoing research lies in the fact that for the first time conceptualization is considered as one of the possible strategies translation of scientific text. The paper attempts to prove that the reconstruction of the original process of conceptualization in the translated text ensures the success of the translation of a scientific text. The work uses new theoretical foundations of translation activity, different from the traditional ones.

The theoretical significance of the dissertation research lies in the fact that the work addresses issues that are important for the development of the theory of translation studies: studying the specifics of scientific translation, substantiating the need to use a special strategy for scientific translation based on conceptualization, identifying the dependence of translation errors on an incorrectly chosen translation strategy. In theoretical terms, it is important to refer to the concepts of a scientific text, scientific communication and conceptualization, which play an important role in developing the foundations of a strategy for the translation of a scientific text.

The practical significance of the work is seen in the possibility of using the results of the analysis in theoretical courses in translation studies, terminology, cognitive linguistics, lexicology, stylistics, rhetoric. The main provisions and conclusions regarding the choice of a scientific translation strategy can be applied in practical translation classes.

The research material was the original scientific monographs and articles in English and their translations into Russian: D.Crystal "English as a global language" (1997), D.Crystal " English language as global" (2001) (translated by N.V. Kuznetsova), N.Chomsky "Language and problems of knowledge" (1997), N.Chomsky "Language and problems of knowledge" (1999) (translated by I.M. Kobozeva, N. .Iskadze, A.A. Arefieva), M.Black "Metaphor" (1962), M.Black "Metaphor" (1990) (translated by M.A. Dmitrovskaya), D.Davidson "What metaphors mean" (1980), D. Davidson “What do metaphors mean” (1990) (translated by M.A. Dmitrovskaya), John R. Searle “Metaphor” (1980), J. Searle “Metaphor” (1990) (translated by V.V. Turovskaya), and See also originals of scientific articles in Russian and their translations into English: V.V. Nalimov "Science and Biosphere: An Experience of Comparing Two Systems" (1970), V.V. Nalimov Science and the biosphere. An attempt at a comparative study of the two systems” (1981), V.V. Nalimov “On a certain parallel between the Bohr complementarity principle and the metaphorical structure of ordinary language” (1976), V.V. Nalimov "On some parallels between the Bohr complementarity principle and the metaphoric structure of ordinary language" (1981), B.B. Nalimov "Sorrow for the Lost Integrity" (1979), V.V. Nalimov "The penetration of the humanities into other fields of knowledge" (1981) (translated by JI.P. Moshanskaya).

A total of 1053 pages of printed text were analyzed. 4258 contexts were subjected to a detailed analysis, obtained as a result of a continuous and targeted selection from the body of practical material.

Methods are determined by the purpose and objectives of the research. The main method of dissertation research is the method of comparative analysis. In the ongoing research, general scientific methods are used: methods of analysis, synthesis, comparison, classification, observation and description. In addition to general scientific methods, private methods are used: conceptual, textual, contextual types of analysis and modeling method.

The main provisions for defense:

1. Conceptualization as the basis of a scientific text determines the strategy of its translation. Modeling by the translator of the process of conceptualization of the source text is based on the identification of meaning-bearing supporting components of the text.

2. The logical relationships between the supporting words of the original scientific text are a kind of "marks", the totality of which corresponds to the mental model of this text.

3. Relationships between key words expressed in a scientific text with the help of basic words, explicatives and associatives are represented by the following types of logical relations: genus-species, species-generic, partitive, process-result and cause-effect relations.

4. There is an analogy between the types of logical relations in the source and translated texts. The conceptual space of the original scientific text, organized on the basis of logical relations, has such a property as integrity, the translation of which is mandatory in scientific translation.

5. Reconstruction of the original logical relations in the translated text ensures the success of the translation strategy of the scientific text. Incorrect translation by the translator of the initial types of logical relations leads to logical errors that distort the scientific knowledge contained in the original text.

Approbation of work. The main results of the study were discussed at international scientific conferences: "The Changing Language World" (Perm, 2001), "Language of Professional Communication" (Chelyabinsk, 2003), "English in a Multi-Cultural Community" (Izhevsk, 2005), "Vocabulary, lexicography, terminography in Russian, American and other cultures" (Ivanovo, 2005), "Problems of language functioning in different areas speech communication (on the 80th anniversary of Professor M.N. Kozhina)" (Perm, 2005), "Linguistics, translation and intercultural communication" (Yekaterinburg, 2005), at interuniversity scientific and practical conferences: "Linguistic / psycholinguistic problems of learning a second language ”(Perm, 2003), “Linguistic Consciousness and Text” (Perm, 2005) and at conferences of young scientists: “Problems of Philology” (Perm, 2003), “Problems of Philology and Teaching Philological Disciplines” (Perm, 2004). 11 papers have been published on the topic of the dissertation.

Work structure. The dissertation consists of an introduction, two chapters, a conclusion and a list of references. The material is illustrated with sixteen tables and three figures.

Similar theses in the specialty "Comparative-historical, typological and comparative linguistics", 10.02.20 VAK code

  • Lacunas in a literary text: a linguoculturological study: Based on the literary works of French-speaking European writers 2001, candidate of philological sciences Sitkareva, Irina Kimovna

  • Strategies for Linguistic and Cultural Adaptation of a Literary Text in Translation 2010, candidate of philological sciences Voynich, Irina Vladimirovna

  • Stylistic means of creating a linguocultural image of an ideal hero in the original text and in translation: based on the works of J. Fleming 2013, candidate of philological sciences Savchenko, Elena Pavlovna

  • Structural levels of equivalence in special translation: on the material of the English-Russian journalistic translation 2009, candidate of philological sciences Poluboyarova, Marina Vladimirovna

  • Linguistic and cognitive aspects of the translation of anthropocentric conceptual metaphorical models: on the material of English fiction of the late XIX - early XX century 2011, candidate of philological sciences Evstafova, Yana Anatolyevna

Dissertation conclusion on the topic "Comparative-historical, typological and comparative linguistics", Kharitonova, Elena Anatolyevna

CONCLUSIONS TO CHAPTER II

1. The main task of the methodology for analyzing the original scientific text was to identify conceptualization based on the types of logical relations. The following provisions are the methodological principles of scientific text analysis: 1) conceptualization as the basis of a scientific text is updated with the help of logical relations; 2) the types of logical relationships between key words are similar to the logical relationships between scientific concepts. The lack of the possibility of direct observation of the course of conceptualization determined the need to model this process on the basis of logical relationships.

2. A comparative analysis of scientific monographs, articles and their translations revealed two types of mistakes made by translators: replacing the original types of logical relations with other types and excluding the original logical relations from the translation. A comparative analysis of the monographs D.Crystal "English as a global language", N.Chomsky "Language and problems of knowledge" and their translations into Russian showed that the largest number of cases of incorrect translation of the original logical relations is associated with the incorrect transmission of species concepts (22, 9% in the monograph by D. Kristal, 16.1% in the monograph by N. Chomsky), which led to an unjustified increase in the level of theoreticality of the original text.

3. In both monographs, cases of incorrect translation of logical relations were revealed, expressed in the replacement of a specific concept by a generic one (6.9% in the monograph by D. Kristal, 12.1% in the monograph of N. Chomsky), as well as in the exclusion from the translation of the concept of the species ( 16% in the monograph by D. Kristal, 4% in the monograph by N. Chomsky). These facts indicate that the translators of monographs are "ahead" of the author's thought, trying to generalize the knowledge they have received. The generalization of the acquired knowledge led to a violation of the logic of the original text.

4. A comparative analysis of scientific articles and their translations into English and Russian revealed the following types of substitutions for logical relations:

Replacing the concept of species with the concept of genus;

The concept of the genus by the concept of the species;

The concept of the whole is the concept of a part;

Concepts of a part-whole by genus-species concepts;

Concepts of process by concept of result;

Exclusion from translation in the PT of the species, genus and part.

5. The greatest number of errors in the translation of scientific articles was made by translators during the translation of aspect ratios (62%). Errors in the translation of the original species relations resulted in an increase in the theoretical level of the original text and, as a result, a distortion of the original author's concept. The smallest number of translation errors was made in the translation of the concepts of part-whole (6%) and process-result (2%). The conducted comparative analysis of IT and PT proved the possibility of using the interpretation of logical relations as a factor in the methodology and strategy for the translation of a scientific text.

CONCLUSION

At the end of the 20th century, certain doubts about the possibilities of the equivalent translation strategy were established in the theory of translation studies. The reason for this is that the main principle of the development of translation studies, as well as linguistics in general, is anthropocentrism. Per recent times the concept of translation methods, including the translation of a scientific text, has changed significantly due to new principles of analysis that have updated and enriched the traditional concept of translation as a linguistic activity. Modern translation studies are in the process of transition from general translation strategies to particular methods of translation. This dissertation research is devoted to the study of conceptualization as the main typological property of a scientific text, which allows developing a strategy for the translation of a scientific type of text.

We tried to show that the study of the process of conceptualization in a scientific text is one of the possible ways to study the nature of a scientific text and develop, on this basis, a methodology and strategy for its translation. The translation strategy of a scientific text in the work is substantiated by linguistic, semiotic, cognitive and communicative factors. In the aspect of linguistics, the translation of a scientific text is a translation of logical relationships between key words (basic words, explicatives and associatives) that verbalize the author's concept. In the aspect of semiotics, the translation of a scientific text is characterized by the venative arrangement of certain types of sign, which makes it possible to reconstruct the course of the author's thought. In the cognitive aspect, the translation of a scientific text is a "rediscovery" by the translator of the author's concept by recreating the original cognitive model of knowledge. In the aspect of communication, the translation of a scientific text is a two-stage process of understanding and translating special knowledge.

One of the results of the study was the realization that, within the framework of translation theory, it became possible to present the translation of a scientific text not as a search for linguistic correspondences, but as a process of interpretation, understanding and reconstruction by the translator of the original author's knowledge model based on logical relationships between concepts.

The choice of the analysis of the logical relations underlying conceptualization as the main factor in the strategy of scientific translation is motivated by the fact that these relations exist objectively, are expressed explicitly, and therefore can serve as a kind of reference points and criteria for adequacy in the translation methodology. With all the richness of interpretations of a scientific text, the reproduction of logical relations seems to us mandatory. If you stand on given point view, then it is necessary to recognize that knowledge of the language does not dominate over individual consciousness in scientific translation. Its form and semantics do not allow the translator to fully overcome the conceptual barrier of a special text.

For a long time, the strategy of translation of a scientific text was based on the main principle - the identification of language equivalents. The concept of strategy, as well as some other categories of translation studies, was built mainly taking into account language difficulties. One of the widespread traditional ideas about the method of translation of a scientific text is that individual words - terms are recognized as the object of translation analysis. The property of the integrity of the scientific text and scientific knowledge was not taken into account by the researchers of translation.

The main task set before us in the study was to comprehensively substantiate the hypothesis about the analogous nature of conceptualization in IT and PT. The methodology of analysis and the entire logical mechanism for constructing the work were subordinated to the same task. A feature of the research methodology of conceptualization in the study is that the focus is on the integrity of the scientific text. The process of conceptualization that we are studying, which underlies the scientific text, meets the requirements of the logic of the development of scientific knowledge, since in each individual case the scientist sets himself the goal of verbalizing knowledge in such a way as to be understood and thereby objectify individual scientific knowledge.

The conducted comparative analysis of IT and PT proved the possibility of using the interpretation of logical relations as a factor in the translation strategy. Genus-species, species-generic, part-whole, process-result, cause-effect and subject-attribute relations were analyzed. In the course of the study, a corpus of translation errors made by translators was identified, their description and classification were carried out. It has been established that the largest number of cases of incorrect translation of the original logical relations is associated with the transmission of species concepts. Replacing the original specific concept with a generic one in the translated text (22.9% in the monograph by D. Kristal, 16.1% in the monograph by N. Khomsky, 62% in scientific articles) leads to an increase in the theoretical nature of the original scientific text, which distorts the original author's knowledge.

Translators of a scientific text generally make two types of errors: 1) replacing the original logical relations with others; 2) exclusion of the original logical relations from the translation. The most typical substitutions are: the replacement of the concept of a species by the concept of a genus, the concept of a genus by the concept of a species, the concept of the whole by the concept of a part, the concept of a part-whole by genus-species concepts, the concept of a process by the concept of a result. The most typical logical concepts, excluded by translators from translation into PT, are the concepts of species, genus, and part. The analysis of scientific texts showed that these types of errors lead to a distortion of the meaning of the original text, which is expressed in an increase or decrease in the level of theoreticality of the original scientific text.

In the course of a comparative analysis of IT and TP, cases of incorrect translation of the original logical relations were identified and analyzed, which is expressed in the following: 1) when the specific concept is replaced by a generic one, as well as when the original specific concept is excluded from the translation, the level of theoreticality of the TP increases; 2) when a generic concept is replaced by a specific one, as well as when the original specific concept is excluded from the translation, the level of theoreticality of the TP decreases. In the first case, the translator “outstrips” the author’s thought, trying to generalize the knowledge he has received, in the second case, the translator “thinks” for the author of the original, trying to specify the original meaning. In both cases, this leads to a distortion of the logic of the source text and the author's concept.

Based on the results of the study, it can be concluded that the leading role in the translation of a scientific text belongs to the successful translation of the results of conceptualization, which is based on the types of logical relations. Prospects for further research on translation may be the problem of choosing translation strategies in other types of translation.

List of references for dissertation research candidate of philological sciences Kharitonova, Elena Anatolyevna, 2006

1. Averbukh K.Ya. General theory of the term. - Ivanovo: Publishing house Ivan, state. un-ta, 2004. -252 p.

2. Aksenova G.N. Cognitive strategies of argumentative speech activity: Dissertation. . candida philol. Sciences. Barnaul, Altai State those. un-t.2003. - 192 p.

3. Alekseeva L.M. term and metaphor. Perm, Publishing house Perm. unta. 1998i.-250 p.

4. Alekseeva L.M. Metaphorization as a means of dialogic scientific text // Anthropocentric approach to language. Part I. Perm, Perm. un-t. 1998 -FROM. 89-120.

5. Alekseeva L.M. Stereotypical and individual in scientific metaphor // Stereotyping and creativity in the text: Interuniversity. Sat. scientific works. Perm, Perm. un-t. 1999. - S. 161-179.

6. Alekseeva L.M. Intellectuality versus informationality in the term. Current problems and thermal specialists TA termshographp. Abstracts of the supplementary international scientific conference // Kshv r.2000 2-4 fierce // Ukladach Z.U.Borisova. K.: KMUCA, 2000. - S. 136-137.

7. Alekseeva L.M. On the specifics of the translation of a scientific text // Stereotyping and creativity in the text: Interuniversity. Sat. scientific works. Perm, Perm. un-t. 2001.-S. 63-73.

8. Alekseeva L.M. Specificity of scientific translation: Proc. special course allowance. Perm, Perm. un-t. 2002i. - 125 p.

9. Alekseeva L.M. Metaphors we choose // With love for language. Moscow-Voronezh, Institute of Languages ​​RAS. Voronezh, Mrs. un-t. 2002 - S. 288-298.

10. Alekseeva L.M., Mishlanova S.L. Medical discourse: theoretical basis and principles of analysis. Perm, Publishing house Perm. university 2002z. - 200 s.

11. Alekseeva L.M. From "Traduce" to "Interpret" in scientific translation // Stereotyping and creativity in scientific translation: Interuniversity. Sat. scientific works. Perm, Perm. un-t. 20024. - S. 78-91.

12. Alekseeva L.M. Anthropological factor in scientific translation // Bulletin of the Perm University. Foreign languages and literature. Linguistics and communication. Issue. 4. Perm, Perm. state unt. 20042. - S. 42-46.

13. Alekseeva L.M. The concept of an individual cognitive model of a scientific text // Stereotyping and creativity in the text: Interuniversity. Sat. scientific works. Issue. 9 (based on the materials of the International Scientific Conf.). - Perm, Perm. un-t. 2005i. pp. 4-10.

14. Alekseeva L.M. From transformational theories to translating personality // Interpretation. Understanding. Translation. M., St. Petersburg: Publishing House of St. Petersburg State University of Economics, 2005. - S. 28-43.

15. Anthropocentric approach to language: Interuniversity. Sat. scientific tr.: in 2 hours. Part II.-Perm, vocational school. 1998.-S. 103-193.

16. Aristotle. Analysts one and two. M., State publishing house of political literature. 1952. 438 p.

17. Aristotle. Works in four volumes, vol. 4. Academy of Sciences of the USSR. Institute of Philosophy. -M., Publishing House "Thought". 1984. 890 p.

18. Askoldov S.A. Concept and word // Russian literature. From the theory of literature to the structure of the text. Anthology. M., Academy. 1997. -S. 267-279.

19. Babenko L.G. Philological analysis of the text. Fundamentals of theory, principles and aspects of analysis. M., Academic project. -Yekaterinburg, Business book. 2004. - 464 p.

20. Babenko L.G., Kazarin Yu.V. Workshop. M., Academic project. - Ekaterinburg, Business book. 2003. - 400 p.

21. Bazhenova E.A. Methods of explication and functions of someone else's speech in a scientific text // Functional varieties speech in the communicative aspect: Interuniversity. Sat. scientific tr. Perm, Perm. un-t. 1988.-S. 92-97.

22. Bazhenova E.A. Polytextuality of the scientific text // Stereotyping and creativity in the text. Perm, Perm. state un-t. 2000. - S. 66-90.

23. Bazhenova E.A. Reflexive structures in the scientific text // Stereotyping and creativity in the text. Perm, Perm. state un-t. 2002. - S. 164-185.

24. Bart R. From science to literature // Selected Works. Semiotics. Poetics. -M., Progress. 1994. S. 375-383.

25. Barkhudarov L.S. Language and translation. Issues of general and particular theory of translation. M., "International Relations". 1975. -239 p.

26. Bakhtin M.M. Aesthetics of verbal creativity. - M., Art. 1979. 423 p.

27. Bibikhin V.V. Word and event. M., Editorial URSS. 2001.-280 p.

28. Blakar R. Language as a tool social power// Language and modeling of social interaction. M., Progress. 1987. - S. 88-126.

29. Bogin G.I. Philological hermeneutics. Kalini, Kalinin state. un-t. 1982. - 86 p.

30. Bogin G.I. The model of linguistic personality in its relation to the varieties of texts: Abstract of the thesis. dis. . Dr. Philol. Sciences. D., 1984. -354 p.

31. Bogin G.I. Schemes of the reader's actions in understanding the text: A tutorial. Kalinin, KGU. 1989i. - 70 s.

32. Bogin G.I. Criticism of translation in the light of modern ideas about reflection // Translation as a process and result: language, culture, psychology: Sat. scientific tr. Kalinin, Kalinin, state. un-t. 19892. -S. 7-14.

33. Boldyrev N.N. Cognitive Semantics: Lecture Course in English Philology. Tambov, TGU Publishing House named after G. R. Derzhavin. Ed. 3rd, sr. 2002.- 123 p.

34. Borisova JI.M. Pragmatic aspects of the translation of text-forming speech actions in artistic discourse // Translation: language and culture: Materials of the international. scientific conf. Voronezh, TsChKI. 2000.-S. 20-21.

35. Brevdo I.F. Disambiguation mechanisms in a joke: Dis. . candida philol. Sciences. Tver, Publishing house Tver. state university 1999. -171 p.

36. Weinrich X. Linguistics of lies // Language and modeling of social interaction. M., Progress. 1987. - S. 44-88.

37. Vasiliev L.G. Translator in the communicative space // Translation as modeling and translation modeling: Sat. scientific tr. -Tver, TSU. 1991. -S. 25-32.

38. Vasiliev L.G. Linguistic aspects of understanding: Dis. . doc. philol. Sciences. Kaluga, Publishing House of Kaluga University. 1999. - 251 p.

39. Vezhbitskaya A. Language. Culture. Cognition. M., "Russian Dictionaries", 1996.-416 p.

40. Vernadsky V.I. Biosphere and noosphere. - M., Science. 1989. -258 p.

41. Vinokur G.O. Language culture. Essays on linguistic technology. M., Publishing house "Worker of education". 1925. - 215 p.

42. Volodina M.N. Cognitive-informational nature of the term. -M., Publishing House of Moscow State University. 2000. 128 p.

43. Volodina M.N. Information-conceptual status of the term and its cognitive-definitional significance // Scientific and technical terminology. Scientific-tech. abstract: Sat. materials of the 10th int. scientific conf. in treminology. Issue. 1. M., VNIIKI. 2004. -S. 15-16.

44. Vygotsky JI.C. Collected works in 6 volumes, v.2. Problems of general psychology. M., Pedagogy. 1982. - 504 p.

45. Vygotsky JI.C. Collected works in six volumes, v.6. scientific legacy. M., Pedagogy. 1984i. - 400 s.

46. ​​Vygotsky JI.C. Collected works in six volumes, v.4. -M., Pedagogy. 1984 432 p.

47. Wüster E. International language standardization in technology // History of domestic terminology. v.2. Directions and methods of terminological research. Book 2. M., Moscow Lyceum. 1999.-S. 202-220.

48. Gadamer G.G. Truth and Method: Fundamentals of Philosophical Hermeneutics. M., Progress. 1988. 699 p.

49. Galeeva H.JI. Parameters of the typology of literary texts in the activity theory of translation: Dis. . doc. philol. Sciences. - Tver, Publishing House of the Tver-th University. 1999. 396 p.

50. Galeeva N.JI. Dynamic factors of translation // Language subsystems: stability and movement: Sat. scientific tr. Tver, Tver. state unt. 2002.-S. 30-40.

51. Galkina O.V. Metaphor as a tool of cognition (on the material of terms-metaphors of a computer interface): Dis. . candida philol. Sciences. Tver, Publishing house Tver. state university 2004. - 141 p.

52. Hegel G.W.F. Science of logic. M., Publishing house "Thought". 1998.-1072 p.

53. Heisenberg V. Steps beyond the horizon. M., Progress. 1987. - 368s.

54. Gerd A.S. Formation of the terminological structure of the Russian biological text. D., Leningrad University Press. 1981. 112 p.

55. Glebova A.N. On some communicative aspects of scientific text translation // Translation: language and culture. Materials of the international scientific conf. Voronezh CCHKI. 2000. - S. 21-22.

56. Goncharova E.A. Text interpretation. German. M., Higher school. 2005. - 368 p.

57. Gorodetsky B.Yu. Terminology and translation studies // Translation and linguistics of the text: collection of articles. M., Higher school. 1994. S. 54-63.

58. Gorsky D.P. The question of abstraction and the formation of concepts. -M., Publishing House of the Academy of Sciences of the USSR. 1961. 351 p.

59. Gorsky D.P. Generalization and knowledge. M., Thought. 1985. -208 p.

60. Greimas A.Zh. Structural semantics: The search for a method. M., Academic project. 2004. -368 p.

61. Humboldt W. von Selected works on linguistics. M., Progress. 1984. - 397 p.

62. Humboldt W. von Linguistics and Philosophy of Culture. -M., Progress. 1985.-451 p.

63. Gusev S.S., Pukshansky B.Ya. Ordinary mindset. Structure and methods of organization. St. Petersburg, "Nauka". 1994 - 87

64. Dyck T.A. van language. Cognition. Communication. M., Progress. 1989.-312 p.

65. Dyck T.A. van, Kinch V. Strategies for understanding a connected text // New in foreign linguistics. Issue 23. Cognitive aspects of language.-M., Progress. 1988.-S. 153-211.

66. Dyck T.A. van, Kinch V. Macrostrategies // Language. Cognition. Communication. M., Progress. 1989. - S. 41 -93.

67. Demyankov V.Z. Concept and concept in fiction and scientific language // Questions of Philology. No. 1. M., 200li. - S. 35-47.

68. Demyankov V.Z. Linguistic interpretation of the text: universal and national (idio-ethnic strategies) // Language and culture: Facts and values: to the 70th anniversary of Yu.S. Stepanova. M., Languages Slavic culture. 2001 - S. 309-323.

69. Derrida J. Essay on the name. Moscow, Institute of Experimental Sociology. SPb. Aletheia. 1998. - 192 p.

70. Dianova G.A. Term and concept: problems of evolution. Moscow, Euroschool. 2000. - 184 p.

71. Dovbysh O.V. English financial terminology and problems of its translation into Russian (on the basis of annual financial reports of foreign companies): Dis. . candida philol. Sciences. M., Mosk. state oblaet, u.s. 2003. - 186 p.

72. Drozdova T.V. Representation of concepts in a scientific text (ordinary versus scientific) // Text and Discourse: Traditional and Cognitive-Functional Aspects of Research. Ryazan, Ryazan state. ped. un-t. 2002. - S. 124-128.

73. Drozdova T.V. Problems of understanding the scientific text (English-language economic texts). Astrakhan, Astrakhan. state those. un-t. Publishing house ASTU. 2003. - 224 p.

74. Davidson D. Truth and interpretation. M., Praxis. 2003. -448 p.

75. Evteev S.V. The problem of modeling the linguo-culturological context in the translation of a scientific text. Dis. . candida philol. Sciences. M., 2003. - 203 p.

76. Yeger G. Communicative and functional equivalence // Questions of the theory of translation in foreign linguistics: Sat. Art. M., International relations. 1978.-S. 137-157.

77. Eltsova L.F. The term in scientific communication // Scientific and technical terminology. Scientific and technical abstract collection. Issue. 2. M., VNIIKI. 2002. - S. 26-27.

78. Emuzova E.A. Intercultural communication in the field of science: Dis. . candida philol. Sciences. Nalchik, Kabardino-Balkarian state. un-t. 2004.-160 p.

79. Zheltukhina M.R. Tropological suggestiveness of mass media discourse: on the problem of the speech impact of tropes in the language of the media: Monograph. M., Institute of Linguistics RAS. - Volgograd, Publishing House of VF MUPC. 2003. - 656 p.

80. Zhinkin N.I. Speech as a conductor of information. M., Publishing house "Science". 1982. - 156 p.

81. Jolls K.K. Thought. Word. Metaphor. Problems of semantics in philosophical illumination. Kyiv, Naukovka Dumka. 1984. - 304 p.

82. Zadorina V.G. Linguistic mechanisms of conceptualization // Problems of Philology: Proceedings of Conf. young scientists and students (April 2002). Perm, Perm. un-t. 2003. - S. 44-45.

83. Zalevskaya A.A. Individual "knowledge base" and the problem of translation // Translation as a process and result: language, culture, psychology: Sat. scientific tr. Kalinin, Kalinin, state. un-t. 1989. - S. 29-35.

84. Zalevskaya A.A. Mechanisms of metaphorization and their consideration in order to model the author's projection of the text in translation // Translation as modeling and modeling of translation: Sat. scientific tr. Tver, TSU. 1991. -S. 69-82.

85. Zalevskaya A.A. Text and its understanding. Tver, Tver state. un-t. 2001. - 177 p.

86. Zalevskaya A.A. Some problems of the theory of text understanding // Questions of linguistics №3. 2002. S. 62-68.

87. Kade O. Problems of translation in the light of the theory of communication // Questions of the theory of translation in foreign linguistics: Sat. Art. M., International relations. 1978. - S. 69-91.

88. Kant I. Criticism of pure reason. M., Thought. 1994i. 591 p.

89. Kant I. Collected works in eight volumes, vol. 4. Prolegomomons. Fundamentals of the metaphysics of morals. Metaphysical principles of natural science. Critique of practical reason. Moscow, Choro Publishing House. 1994 -630 s.

90. Karasik V.I., Slyshkin G.G. Linguistic and cultural concept as a unit of research // Methodological problems of cognitive linguistics: Sat. scientific tr. Voronezh, VSU. 2001. - S. 75-80.

91. Karasik V.I. Language circle: personality, concepts, discourse. Volgograd, Change. 2002. - 777 p.

92. Karasik V.I. Aspects of linguistic personality // Problems of speech communication: Interuniversity. Sat. scientific tr. Saratov, Sarat Publishing House. unta. 2003.-S. 96-106.

93. Karaulov Yu.N. Russian language and linguistic personality. M., Publishing house "Science". 1987, - 263 p.

94. Kasevich V.B. Language structures and cognitive activity // Language and cognitive activity. M., Institute of Linguistics of the Academy of Sciences of the USSR. 1989. - S. 8-18.

95. Cassirer E. The power of metaphor // Theory of metaphor. M., Progress. 1990. - S. 33-44.

96. Katsnelson S.D. The content of the word, meaning and designation. -M., Leningrad., Publishing house "Nauka". 1965. 108 p.

98. Kemerov V.E. Strategy // Modern Philosophical Dictionary. 2nd ed. London, Frankfurt, Paris, Luxembourg, Moscow, Minsk, Panprint. 1998. - S. 865-867.

99. Klimenko O.K. Typology of scientific text // Problems of text typology: Sat. scientific and analytical reviews M., 1984. - S. 96107.

100. Klyukanov I.E. Dynamics of intercultural communication: to the construction of a new conceptual apparatus: Dis. . doc. philol. Sciences. -Tver, Publishing house of the Tvers-go state. university 1999. 395 p.

101. Kozhina M.N. Comparative study of the scientific style and some trends in its development during the scientific and technological revolution // Language and Style scientific literature. M., "Science". 1977. - S. 9-25.

102. Kozhina M.N., Rychkova N.V. Expression of connectivity as a criterion for intra-style differentiation of scientific speech // Functional varieties of speech in the communicative aspect: Interuniversity. Sat. scientific tr. Perm, Perm. un-t. 1988. - S. 16-28.

103. Kozhina M.N. Essays on the history of the scientific style of the Russian literary language of the 18th-20th centuries. in 3 volumes // Development of the scientific style in the aspect of the functioning of language units of various levels, v.1. - Perm, Perm Publishing House. university 1994. part 1. 304 p.

104. Kozhina M.N., Chigovskaya Ya.I. Stylistic and textual status and interaction of the categories of retrospection and prospection in scientific speech // Stereotyping and creativity in the text: Sat. scientific tr. Perm, Perm. state un-t. 2001.-S. 118-157.

105. Kozhina M.N. Speech science and functional stylistics: questions of theory. Selected works. Perm, Perm. state un-t. PSI. PSSGK. 2002.-475 p.

106. Kolegaeva I. Text as a unit of scientific and artistic communication. Odessa, Redak. department, regional press department. 1991.-118s.

107. Komissarov V.N. A word about translation. M., "International Relations". 1973. -215p.

108. Komissarov V.N. Communicative Problems of Translation: Modern Paradigms // Translation and Communication. M., 1997. - S. 6-16.

109. Komlev N.G. Components of the content structure of the word. -M., Editorial URSS. 20031. 192 p.

110. Komlev N.G. Word in speech. denotative aspects. M., Editorial URSS. 2003 - 216 p.

111. Kopanev P.I. Questions of the history and theory of literary translation. Minsk, BGU Publishing House. 1972. - 295 p.

112. Kopanev P.I., Mogilenskikh N.P. On the development of particular theories of scientific and technical translation // Translation and text. Abstracts of reports for the zonal seminar February 9-10, 1989. Penza, 1989. - P. 37-39.

113. Kotyurova M.P. On the concept of connectivity and the means of its expression in Russian scientific speech // Language and style of scientific literature. -M., 1977.-S. 139-158.

114. Kotyurova M.P. On the evolution of the expression of the connectedness of speech in the scientific style XVIII-XIXbb. // Linguistic and stylistic features of the scientific text. -M., Publishing house "Science". 1981. S. 45-60.

115. Kotyurova M.P. Semantic structure of specific peripheral texts // Functional varieties of speech in the communicative aspect: Interuniversity. Sat. scientific tr. Perm, Perm. state un-t. 1988.-S. 4-16.

116. Krasavsky N.A. Emotional concepts in German and Russian linguistic cultures. Volgograd, "Change". 2001. - 495 p.

117. Krasnykh V.V. "Own" among "strangers": myth or reality? -M., ITDGK "Gnosis". 2003. 375 p.

118. Krivonosov A.T. Natural language and logic. Moscow-New York. Wickersham Printing Company. 1993. - 318 p.

119. Krizhanovskaya E.M. Communicative-pragmatic structure of the scientific text: Abstract of the thesis. dis. . cand. philol. Sciences. Perm, 2000.-18 p.

120. Croce B. Theory and history of historiography. M., School "Languages ​​of Russian culture". 1998. - 192 p.

121. Krymsky S.B. Scientific knowledge and principles of its transformation. Kyiv, "Naukova Dumka". 1974. 207 p.

122. Kryukov A.N. Translation as interpretation // Translation and interpretation of the text. M., 1988i. - S. 41-65.

123. Kryukov A.N. Understanding as a translation problem // Translation and interpretation of the text. M., 1988 - S. 65-76.

124. Kryukov A.N. Methodological foundations interpretative concept of translation: Abstract of the thesis. dis. . doc. philol. Sciences. -M., 1989. -42 p.

125. Kryukov A.N. Interlingual communication and the problem of understanding // Translation and communication. M., 1997. - S. 73-83.

126. Kryukova N.F. Metaphorization and metaphoricity as parameters of reflective action in the production and reception of the text: Abstract of the thesis. dis. . doc. philol. Sciences. -M., 2000. - 29 p.

127. Kubryakova E.S. The nominative aspect of speech activity. -M., "Science". 1986.- 158 p.

128. Kubryakova E.S. The role of word formation in the formation of a linguistic picture of the world // The role of the human factor in language: Language and picture of the world. M., 1988. Science. - 212 p.

129. Kubryakova E.S. Evolution of linguistic ideas in the second half of the 20th century (the experience of paradigm analysis) // Language and science of the end of the 20th century. -M., Ros. state humanit. un-t. 1995, pp. 144-238.

130. Kubryakova E.S. Language and knowledge: On the way to gaining knowledge about language: Parts of speech from a cognitive point of view. The role of language in the knowledge of the world. M., Languages ​​of Slavic culture, 2004. - 560 p.

131. Kudasova O.K. The role of stylistic device in the organization of scientific evaluation text // Language and style of scientific presentation. Linguistic research. M., Publishing house "Science". 1983. -S. 34-45.

132. Kukharenko V.A. Text categories in the original and translation of a literary prose work // Translation as a process and result: language, culture, psychology: Sat. scientific tr. Kalinin, Kalinin, state. un-t. 1989.-S. 14-23.

133. Catford JK Linguistic theory of translation // Questions of the theory of translation in foreign linguistics: Sat. Art. M., International relations. 1978. - S. 91-114.

134. Lakoff J., Johnson M. Metaphors we live by // Theory of metaphors.-M., 1990. -S. 387-416.

135. Lakoff J. Thinking in the mirror of classifiers // New in foreign linguistics. Issue 23. Cognitive aspects of language. M., Progress. 1988.-S. 12-52.

136. Lakoff J. Cognitive modeling. From the book "Women, Fire and Dangerous Objects" // Language and Intellect. M., Progress. 1995.-S. 143-184.

137. Lakoff J., Johnson M. Metaphors we live by. -M., Editorial URSS. 2004. 256 p.

138. Lapp L.M. On the emotionality of the scientific text // Functional varieties of speech in the communicative aspect: Interuniversity. Sat. scientific tr. Perm, Perm. state un-t. 1988. - S. 92-97.

139. Lapp L.M. Interpretation of the scientific text in the aspect of the "Speech subject" factor. Publishing house Irkut. university Irkutsk. 1990. - 218 p.

140. Leibniz G.W. New experiences about human mind. M-L., State. social economy. publishing house 1936.-484 p.

141. Leichik V.M. Features of terminology social sciences and spheres of its use // Language and style of scientific presentation. Linguistic research. M., Publishing house "Science". 1983. -S. 70-88.

142. Leichik V.M. Problematics of the terminological theory of the text // Stereotyping and creativity in the text. Perm, Perm. state un-t. 2002. -S. 63-78.

143. Lilova A. Introduction to the general theory of translation. M., Higher school. 1985.-256 p.

144. Likhachev D.S. Conceptosphere of the Russian language // IAN SLYA. 1983. V. 52. No. 1.- S. 3-9.

145. Lotman Yu.M. Analysis of the poetic text. The structure of the verse. - L., "Enlightenment". Leningrad branch. 1972.-271 p.

146. Lotman Yu.M. About the Semiosphere // Uch. Notes of the Tartu State University. Issue 641. Tartu, Tart. state un-t. 1984. - S. 5-23.

147. Lotman Yu.M. Inside the thinking worlds: human text - semiosphere - history. - M., Languages ​​of Russian culture. 1999. - 464 p.

148. Lotman Yu.M. Semiosphere. SPb. St. Petersburg, Art. 2001. - 703 p.

149. Luria A.R. Language and consciousness. M., Publishing house of Moscow State University. 1998. -336 p.

150. Lvovskaya Z.D. The role of extralinguistic sense actualizers in teaching the interpretation of a scientific text // Language and style of scientific presentation. Linguistic research. M., Publishing house "Science". 1983.-S. 195-21

151. McCormack E. Cognitive theory of metaphor // Theory of metaphor. -M., Progress. 1990. S. 358-387.

153. Manerko L.A. The language of modern technology: core and periphery. Monograph. Ryazan, RSPU im. S.A. Yesenin. 2000 - 140 s.

154. Manerko L.A. Fundamentals of conceptual integration of mental spaces // Text and Discourse: traditional and cognitive-functional aspects of research. Ryazan, Ryazan state. ped. unt. 2002.-S. 17-29.

155. Manerko L.A. Terminology in traditional and cognitive-communicative linguistics // Scientific and technical terminology.

156. Scientific and technical. essay. Sat. Proceedings of the 10th Intern. scientific conf. in treminology. Issue. 1. M., VNIIKI. 2004. S. 49-51.

157. Milovidov V.A. Text vs discourse: experience of epistemological analysis // Interpretation. Understanding. Translation: St. Petersburg. SPbGUEF publishing house. 2005.-S. 125-132.

158. Minyar-Beloruchev R.K. General theory of translation and interpretation. M., military publishing house. 1980. - 237 p.

159. Mishlanova SL. Metaphor in the medical text (based on Russian, German, English languages): Dis. . cand. philol. Sciences. Perm, PSU. 1998. - 167 p.

160. Mishlanova SL. Metaphor in medical discourse. Perm, Publishing house Perm. university 2002. - 160 p.

161. Mishlanova SL. Permyakova T.M. Modern concept sphere: trends and perspectives // Stereotyping and creativity in the text: interuniversity. Sat. scientific tr. Perm, Perm. state un-t. 2004. -S. 351-359.

162. Morris C.W. Foundations of the theory of signs // Semiotics, v.1. -Blagoveshchensk, BGK im. A.I. Baudouin de Courtenay. 1998i. pp. 36-83

163. Morris C.W. Signs and actions // Semiotics, v.1. BGK im. A.I. Baudouin de Courtenay. -Blagoveshchensk, 19982. S. 116-127.

164. Motroshilova N.V. Hegel's path to the Science of Logic. Formation of the principles of consistency and historicism. - M., Publishing house "Nauka". 1984.-352 p.

165. Murzin JI. N. Language, text, culture // Human text-culture: Sat. scientific tr. - Ekaterinburg, JSC "Polygraphist". 1994. - S. 160169.

166. Naida Yu.A. To science to translate. Principles of Correspondence // Questions of the Theory of Translation in Foreign Linguistics: Sat. Art. M., International relations. 1978.-S. 114-137.

167. Nalimov V.V. Probabilistic model of language. M., "Nauka", 1979.- 272 p.

168. Nalimov V.V. I scatter my thoughts. On the road and at the crossroads. -M., Progress-Tradition. 2000. 344 p.

169. Nelyubin JI.JI. Explanatory translation dictionary. 3rd ed. M., Flint. The science. 2003. - 320 p.

170. Nikitin M.V. Course of linguistic semantics. St. Petersburg, Scientific Center for Dialogue Problems. 1997. - 760 p.

171. Nikitin M.V. Foundations of cognitive semantics. St. Petersburg. Publishing house RGPU them. A.I. Herzen. 2003. - 277 p.

172. Nikitina S.E. Cultural and linguistic picture of the world in the thesaurus description: Dis. in the form of scientific doc. M., Institute of Linguistics. 1999.-54 p.

173. Novikov A.I. Dominance and transposition in the process of translation // Problems of Applied Linguistics. Sat. scientific Art. M., 2001. -S. 74-79.

174. Ovchinnikova I.G. Associations and utterance: structure and semantics: A textbook for a special course. Perm, Perm. state un-t. 1994.- 124 p.

175. Ortega y Gasset X. Two great metaphors // Theory of Metaphor.- M., Progress. 1990. S. 68-82.

176. Autier-Revue J. Explicit and constitutive heterogeneity: to the problem of the Other in discourse // Quadrature of meaning. M., 1999. - S. 54-82.

177. Pavilenis R.I. The problem of meaning. M., Thought. 1983. - 286s.

178. Panfilov V.Z. Gnoseological aspects of philosophical problems of linguistics. M., "Science". 1982. - 287 p.

179. Translation in the modern world. M., All-Russian translation center. 2001. - 78 p.

180. Translation and linguistics of the text: Sat. Art. M., 1994. - 217 p.

181. Translation and imitation in the literatures of the Middle Ages and the Renaissance. M., IMLI RAN. 2002. - 413 p.

182. Translation and translation studies. Conf. Bashkir, un. Ufa. Bashkir, Mrs. un-t. 1997. - 99 p.

183. Translation as a process and as a result: language, culture, psychology. Kaliningrad, KGU. 1989. - 123 p.

184. Piaget J. Psychogenesis of knowledge and its epistemological significance // Semiotics. Anthology. Ed. 2nd. M., Business book. 200li. -FROM. 98-110.

185. Piaget J. Schemes of action and language acquisition // Semiotics. Anthology. Ed. 2nd. M., Business book. 2001 - P.144-148.

186. Pierce C.S. Elements of logic. Grammatica speculativa // Semiotics, v.1. Blagoveshchensk, BGK im. A.I. Baudouin de Courtenay. 1998. -S. 149-206.

187. Plato. Essay in 3 volumes, v.1. M., Academy of Sciences of the USSR. Publishing house of social economy. liters. Thought. 1968. 622 p.

188. Polanyi M. Personal knowledge. Towards a post-critical philosophy. M., 1985. Progress. - 344 p.

189. Popova Z.D., Sternin I.A. Language and national consciousness. Questions of theory and methodology. Voronezh, Vor. state un-t. 2002. - 314 p.

190. Popper K. The logic of scientific research. M., Publishing house "Respublika". 2004. - 447 p.

191. Potebnya A.A. Aesthetics and poetics. M., Art. 1976. -614 p.

192. Potebnya A.A. theoretical poetics. M., Higher School. 1990.-342 p.

193. Potebnya A.A. Thought and language. Kyiv, SINTO. 1993. - 192 p.

194. Problems of speech communication: interuniversity. Sat. scientific tr. - Saratov, Sarat Publishing House. university 2000. 184 p.

195. Raie K. Classification of texts and methods of translation // Questions of the theory of translation in foreign linguistics: Sat. Art. M., International relations. 1978. - S. 202-225.

196. Razumovsky O.S. Logic of conceptualization and theorization in the context of theory development // Conceptualization and meaning. - Novosibirsk, Science. Sib. dept. 1990.-S. 164-173.

197. Revzin I.I., Rozentsveig V.Yu. Fundamentals of general and machine translation. M., Higher School. 1963.-243 p.

198. Retsker Ya.I. Translation theory and translation practice. M., "International Relations". 1974. - 216 p.

199. Riker P. Metaphorical process as knowledge, imagination and sensation // Theory of metaphor. M., Progress. 1990i. - S. 416-435.

200. Riker P. Living metaphor // Theory of metaphor. M., Progress. 1990-S. 435-356.

201. Richard A. Philosophy of rhetoric // Theory of metaphor. M., Progress. 1990. - S. 44-48.

202. Rogovskaya E.E. Emotional dominant as a structure-forming component of the translation text: Abstract of the thesis. dis. . cand. philol. Sciences. Barnaul, 2004. - 19 p.

203. Rubinstein S.L. Being and consciousness. Man and the world: St. Petersburg: Peter, 2003.-512 p.

204. Ruschakov V.A. Foundations of translation and comparison of languages: Dis. . doc. philol. Sciences. St. Petersburg, Publishing House of St. Petersburg State. university 1997. - 506 p.

205. Ryabtseva N.K. Theory and Practice of Translation: Cognitive Aspect // Translation and Communication. M., Edition of the Russian Academy of Sciences. 1997. - S. 42-63.

206. Ryabtseva N.K. Scientific speech in English. Guide to scientific presentation. Dictionary of turnovers and compatibility of general scientific vocabulary. New dictionary-reference of the active type. M., Flint, Science. 2000. - 598 p.

207. Ryabtseva N.K. Linguistic modeling of natural intelligence and knowledge representation // Problems of applied linguistics. 2001. M., 2002. - S. 228-252.

208. Salimovsky V. A. Genres of speech in functional and stylistic coverage (scientific academic text). Perm, Publishing house Perm. university 2002. - 236 p.

209. Sedov A.I. The history of genetics, captured in the metaphors of its language: quantitative and structural analysis. General principles of analysis. Metaphorical terms // Problems of socio/psycho-linguistics and multilingualism. Issue. 1. M., Moscow Lyceum. 1997. - S. 35-48.

210. Selivanova E.A. Cognitive onomasiology. Kyiv, Publishing house of the Ukrainian phytosociological center. 2000. - 248 p.

211. Semenets O.E., Panasiev A.N. Translation history: account. allowance. - Kyiv, Publishing house at Kyiv, un-te, 1989. 296 p.

212. Semkina N.A. Concepts and their connections in the text // Text and Discourse: traditional and cognitive-functional aspects of research. - Ryazan, Ryazan state. ped. un-t. 2002, pp. 106-114.

213. Sapir E. Language and literature // Selected works on linguistics and cultural studies. M., 1993. - S. 195-203.

214. Serebrennikov B.A. Foreword // The role of the human factor in language: Language and picture of the world. -M., 1988. S. 5-12.

215. Skorokhodko E.F. Lshgvyutichsh bases of automation of the formal search - Kyiv, Naukova Dumka. 1970. 120 p.

216. Slavgorodskaya L.V. On the function of the addressee in scientific prose // Linguistic and stylistic features of the scientific text. - M., Publishing house "Nauka". 1981.-S. 93-103.

217. Slavgorodskaya L.V. On the logical coherence of oral scientific communication // Language and style of scientific presentation. Linguistic research. M., Publishing house "Science". 1983. - S. 34-45.

218. With love for language: Sat. scientific tr. dedicated to E.S. Kubryakova. M. Voronezh, Institute of Linguistics RAS, - Voronezh, state. un-t. 2002. - 492 p.

219. Solganik G.Ya., Dronyaeva T.S. The style of the modern Russian language and the culture of speech. 2nd ed. M., Academy. 2004. - 252 p.

220. Solovieva I.S. The dynamics of meaning in the activity of understanding the text // Language subsystems: stability and movement: Sat. scientific tr. Tver, TGU Publishing House. 2002. - S. 107-112.

221. Solomonik A. Philosophy of sign systems and language. Edition 2nd. M., Mn.: MEG. 2002. - 408 p.

222. Sorokin Yu.A. Intuition and Translation // Translation as Modeling and Modeling of Translation: Sat. scientific tr. Tver, Tver. state un-t. 1991.-S. 4-25.

223. Sorokin Yu.A. Literary translation and its fictitious reality // Compositional semantics. Materials of the third international school-seminar on cognitive linguistics. 18-20 Sept. 2002 Part I. - Tambov, Tambov, state. un-t. 2002, pp. 153-156.

224. Sorokin Yu.A. Translation studies: the status of a translator and psychohermeneutic procedures. M., ITDGK "Gnosis". 2003. - 160 p.

225. Sorokin Yu.A. Is there a literary translation? // Languages ​​and transnational problems. Proceedings of the 1st international scientific conference, v.2. Moscow-Tambov, 2004. - S. 225-230.

226. Saussure F. de Works on linguistics. M., Progress. 1977. -696 p.

227. Saussure F. de Notes on General Linguistics. M., Progress Publishing Group. 2000. - 280 p.

228. Social and human sciences. Patriotic and foreign literature. Ser. 6. Linguistics: RJ / RAS. INION. Center for Humanitarian Scientific Information. Research. Dep. Linguistics. M. 2003-ISNN 02026-2087, 0202-2133. 2003. No. 1. - 203 p.

229. Stepanov Yu.S. Constants: Dictionary of Russian Culture. Research experience M., "Languages ​​of Russian culture." 1997. - 824 p.

230. Stepanov Yu.S. Semiotics of concepts // Semiotics. Anthology edited by Yu.S. Stepanov. Ed. 2nd. M., "Business book". 2001. - S. 603-612.

231. Sternin I.A. Methodology for studying the structure of the concept // Methodological problems of cognitive linguistics. Voronezh, 2001. -S. 58-65.

232. Tatarinov V.A. History of domestic terminology. v.2. Directions and methods of terminological research. Book 2. M., Moscow Lyceum. 1999. - 311 p.

233. Text and discourse: traditional and cognitive-functional aspects of research: Sat. Art. Ryazan, Ryazan state. ped. University named after S.A. Yesenin. 2002. - 236 p.

234. Teliya V.N. Metaphorization and its role in creating a linguistic picture of the world // Metaphor in language and text. M., Science. 1988. S. 26-52.

235. Terminology Issue. 1-3. M., Moscow Lyceum. 1996. -186 p.

236. Translator's notebooks. M., Publishing House of the Institute of International Relations. 1963. - 112 p.

237. Troshina N.N. On the Status of Typological Research in Linguistic Text Theory // Problems of Text Typology: Sat. scientific and analytical reviews. M., 1984. - S. 4-32.

238. Ufimtseva A. A. Lexical meaning: The principle of semiological description of vocabulary. M., Editorial URSS, 2002. - 240 p.

239. Fedorov A.V. Basics general theory translation. M., Higher school. 1983. - 303 p.

240. Feyerabend P. Selected works on the methodology of science. M., Progress. 1986. - 544 p.

241. Fers J. R. Linguistic analysis and translation // Questions of the theory of translation in foreign linguistics: Sat. Art. M., International relations. 1978.-S. 25-36.

242. Fesenko T.A. Ethnomental world of man: experience of conceptual modeling: Dis. . doc. philol. Sciences. -M., Institute of Linguistics Ros. academician Sciences. 1999. 350 p.

243. Fesenko T.A. Conceptual foundations of translation: Proc. allowance. Tambov, Tambov Publishing House. university 2001. - 124 p.

244. Fesenko T.A. Translation in the mirror of cognitive science // With love for language. Sat. scientific tr. dedicated to E.S. Kubryakova. M. Voronezh, 2002. - S. 65-71.

245. Florensky P.A. The term // VYa. No. 1- M., Publishing house "Nauka". 1989.-S. 121-133.

246. Frumkina R.M. Linguist as a cognizing personality // Language and cognitive activity. M., Institute of Linguistics. Academy of Sciences of the USSR. 1989. - S. 52-57.

247. Frumkina R.M. Does modern linguistics have its own epistemology? // Language and science of the late 20th century: Sat. Art. M., All-Russian state. humanitarian university 1995. - S. 74-117.

248. Foucault M. Words and things. Archeology of the Humanities. M., Progress. 1977.-488 p.

249. Fundamental directions of modern American linguistics. Ed. A.A. Kibrika, I.M. Kobozeva, I.A. Sekerina. -M., Publishing House of the Moscow University. 1997. 455 p.

250. Khairullin V.I. Translation of a scientific text (linguoculturological aspect), method, manual. M., 1992. - 127 p.

251. Khairullin V.I. Linguocultural and cognitive aspects of translation: Dis. . doc. philol. Sciences. M., 1995. - 355 p.

252. Cheif WL Memory and verbalization of past experience // Text: aspects of the study of semantics, pragmatics and poetics. M., Editorial URSS. 2001.-342 p.

253. Chenki A. Introduction to the Theory of Cognitive Grammar by R. Langaker // Fundamental Directions of Modern American Linguistics. M., Publishing House of Moscow State University. 1997. - S. 358-359.

254. Chernyavskaya V.E. Intertextuality as a text-forming category in scientific communication (based on German language): Dis. . doc. philol. Sciences. St. Petersburg, 2000. - 352 p.

256. Chudinov A.P. Metaphorical modeling of reality in political narrative // ​​With love for language: Sat. scientific tr. M. - Voronezh, Institute of Linguistics RAS, - Voronezh, state. un-t.2002.-S. 298-310.

257. Shadrin V.I. "Translation Studies" vs. "Translation Studies" // Interpretation. Understanding. Translation. St. Petersburg: Publishing House of St. Petersburg State University of Economics, 2005. P. 43-48.

258. Shapochkin D.V. Cognitive aspects of political discourse (based on British, American and German public speeches): Abstract of the thesis. dis. . cand. philology of sciences. Tyumen, 2005. - 21 p.

259. Schweitzer A.D. Translation theory. Status, problems, aspects. -M., Science. 1988.-214 p.

260. Shevtsova A.A. Metaphorization as a means of meaning formation in the text of a literary fairy tale: Abstract of the thesis. dis. . cand. philol. Sciences. Tver, 2004. - 19 p.

261. Shinkarenkova M.B. metaphorical modeling artistic world in the discourse of Russian rock poetry: Abstract of the thesis. dis. . cand. philol. Sciences. Yekaterinburg, 2005. - 24 p.

262. Shirinkina M.A. Secondary business text and its genre varieties: Abstract of the thesis. dis. . cand. philol. Sciences. Perm, 2001. - 19 p.

263. Shpet G.G. The internal form of the word (Etudes and variations on themes by Humboldt) 2nd ed., M., Editorial URSS. 2003. - 216 p.

264. Shchedrovitsky G.P. Selected works. M., Shk. Cult. Polit. 1995.-759 p.

265. Shchedrovitsky G.P. Philosophy. The science. Methodology. M., Shk. Cult. Politicians. 1997. - 656 p.

266. Shchedrovitsky G.P. Thinking-Understanding-Reflection. M., Heritage MMK. 2005. - 800 p.

267. Eco U. Missing structure. Introduction to semiology. -M., LLP TK Petropolis. 1998. 432 p.

268. Language, discourse and personality: interuniversity. Sat. scientific tr. Tver, TSU. 199.-134 p.

269. Language and science of the late XX century. M., Ros. state humanit. un-t. 1995. -432 p.

270. Jacobson R. Speech communication // Selected works. M., Progress. 1985i.-p. 306-319.

271. Jacobson R. Language in relation to other communication systems // Selected Works. -M., Progress. 1985 pp. 319-331.

272. Jacobson R. On the linguistic aspects of translation // Selected Works. M., Progress. 1985z. - S. 361-369.

273. Alexeiva L., Vdovenko D. La conflictualite du texte scientifique et les problemes de traduction // Synergies Russie. Le pouvoir des mots. Revue de didactologie des langues-cultures. Numero 2. annee 2002. Gerflint. p. 117-125.

274. Carnap R. Empiricism, Semantics, and Ontology // The linguistic turn. University of Chicago Press. 1992. p. 84-112.

275. Fauconnier G. Qualification, roles and domains // Meaning and mental representaions. Indiana University Press. 1988. p. 61-79.

276. Figge U.L. Kognitiv orientierte Lexicographie // Semantik, Lexicographie und Computeranwendungen. Tubingen, 1996. p. 32-46.

277. Kant I. Kritik der reinen Vernunft. In achter Auflage Revidierty v. Dr. Theodor Valentner. Neunte Auflage Lepzig, Verlag der Durr "schen Buchhandlung. 1906. 769 p.

278. Khurshid A. Scientific texts and the evolution of knowledge the need for a critical framework? // Porta Scientiae I. Lingua specialis. Vaasa. The University of Vaasa. 2002. p. 123-134.

279. Lakoff G., Johnson G. Metaphors we live by. Chicago-London. 1980. 120 p.

280. Lyons J. Natural language and universal grammar. Essays in Linguistic Theory. Volume 1. Cambridge University Press. 1991. 290 p.

282. Newmark P. No global communication without translation // Translation today. Clevedon. buffalo. Toronto. Sydney. 2003.p. 55-67.

283. Nida A. Toward a science of Translating. CH. 2. Liden. 1964. 1861. P

284. Picht H. The Concept in Terminology: a unit of thought, knowledge or cognition? // Scientific and technical terminology. Scientific and technical abstract collection. Issue 2., M., 2002. 71 p.

285. Porta Scientiae I. Lingua specialis. Veera Puro-Aho. Vaasa., 2002. 118 p.

286. Smith V. The literal meaning of Lexical items. Some theoretical considerations on the semantics of complex and Transferred Nominals with special reference to Danish and Russian. Copenhagen. Working pages in LSP. 7-1999Y2000. 105p.

287. Shank R., Kass A. Knowledge representation in people and machines // Meaning and mental representaions. Indiana University Press. 1988. p. 181-199.

288. Surowaniec J. Theoretical foundations of Logopaedic Terminology. 1997. http://www.wsp.krakow.pl/-szydelko/slownik/index.htm.

289. Talmy L. The relation of Grammar to Cognition // Topics in Cognitive Linguistics. Osten: Ed. by B. Rudzka-Osten, 1988. p. 162-205.

290. Turner M., Fauconnier G. Blending and grammar, 4th Int. Cognitive Linguistics Conf. 1995. http//www.wam.umd.edu/-mturn/.

291. Turner M., Fauconnier G. Principles of Conceptual Integration, 2nd Conference on Conceptual Structure, Discourse, and Language, 1996. http://www.wam.umd.edu/-mturn/.

292. Turner M., Fauconnier G. Conceptual intergartion Networks. Cognitive Science Society, Inc. Expanded web version 2001, 61 p. http://blending.stanford.edu.

293. Weissenhofer P. Conceptology in Terminology Theory, Semantics and Word-Formation. Vienna: TermNet, 1995. 270 p.1. DICTIONARIES

294. Concise Dictionary cognitive terms. Ed. Kubryakova E.S., Demyankova V.Z., Pankratsa Yu.G., Luzina L.G. M., Publishing house of Moscow State University. 1996.-245 p.

295. Ozhegov S.I. Dictionary of the Russian language. 17th ed. M., Russian language. 1985.-797 p.

296. Stylistic encyclopedic dictionary of the Russian language. Under. ed. M.N. Kozhina. M.: Flinta. The science. 2003. 696 p.

297. Dictionary of foreign words. M., Russian language. 1989. - 624 p.

298. Soviet encyclopedic dictionary. 2nd ed. M., Soviet Encyclopedia. 1984. - 1600 p.

299. Philosophical dictionary. 5th ed. M., Politizdat. 1986. - 590 p.

300. Philosophical encyclopedic dictionary. - M., INFRA-M. 1997.-576 p.

301. Collins Cobuild English Language Dictionary. Harper Collins Publishers. 1993. 1703 p.

302. New Webster's Dictionary of the English Language. College Edition. Sur jeet Publications. Fourth Reprint. USA. 1988. 1824 p.

303. Oxford Concise Dictionary of Linguistics. Oxford. new york. Oxford University Press. 1997. 410 p.

304. SOURCES OF LANGUAGE MATERIAL

305. Black M. Metaphor // Models and metaphors studies in language and philosophy, Itaca; London; Cornell University Press. 1962, p. 25-47.

306. Crystal D. English as a global language. Cambridge University Press. 1997. p. 202.

307. Chomsky N. Language and problems of knowledge. Massachusetts; London; England: The MIT Press Cambridge, 1997. p. 199.

308. Davidson D. What metaphors mean // Reference, truth and reality: Essay on the Philosophy of Language. London, 1980. p. 238-254.

309. Nalimov V.V. Science and the Biosphere. An attempt at a Comparative study of the two systems // Faces of Science. Philadelphia, Pa: ISI Press. 1981. p. 145-158.

310. Nalimov V.V. On some parallels between the Bohr Complementarity Principle and the Metaphoric Structure of Ordinary language // Faces of Science. Philadelphia, Pa: ISI Press. 1981. p. 139-143.

311. Nalimov V.V. The penetration of the humanities into other fields of knowledge//Faces of Science. Philadelphia, Pa: ISI Press. 1981. p. 199-209.

312. Searle John R. Metaphor // Metaphor and thought. Cambridge, 1980. p. 92-124.

313. Black M. Metaphor // Theory of metaphor. M., Progress. 1990. - S. 153-172.

314. Davidson D. What do metaphors mean // Theory of metaphor. M., Progress. 1990.-S. 173-193.

315. Crystal D. English as a global language. M., the whole world. 2001.-238 p.

316. Nalimov V.V. Science and the biosphere: the experience of comparing two systems // Priroda. 1970. No. 11. S. 55-63.

317. Nalimov V.V. On some parallel between Bohr's principle of complementarity and the metaphorical structure of ordinary language // The principle of complementarity and materialistic dialectics. M., 1976. -S. 121-123.

318. Nalimov V.V. Sorrow for the lost integrity // Knowledge is power. 1979. No. 5.-S. 21-23.

319. Searl J. Metaphor // Theory of metaphor. M., Progress. 1990.-S. 173-193.

320. Chomsky N. Language and problems of knowledge. - Blagoveshchensk, BGK im. A.I. Baudouin de Courtenay. 1999. 252 p.

Please note that the scientific texts presented above are posted for review and obtained through original dissertation text recognition (OCR). In this connection, they may contain errors related to the imperfection of recognition algorithms. There are no such errors in the PDF files of dissertations and abstracts that we deliver.

Speaking of cognitive information, we mean information that is born in the process of perception and cognition of the world, "all those data that come to a person from the outside through various sensory-perceptual and sensory-motor channels, as well as those data that have already been processed by the central nervous system, internalized and reinterpreted by a person and presented in his head in the form of mental representations" (Kubryakova, KSKT, 35). The cognitive structures "stored" in memory mentally represent the external world, and the models developed by researchers, in turn, represent and reflect this mental world. In cognitive linguistics, various cognitive structures and processes in the human mind can act as explanatory model constructs: frames and scenarios (Minsky, 1979; Fillmore, 1975), propositional models (Johnson-Laird 1983; Paivio 1986; Arutyunova 1976), a cognitive model as characterization of the categorization process in natural language (Lakoff, 1987), ideal semantic models (Kamenskaya, 1990), mental spaces (Fauconnier, 1998; 1999), concept and mental lexicon (Kubryakova 1991; KSCT 1996).

A new stage in the development of cognitive linguistics was the development of J. Fauconnier's theory of mental spaces (Fauconnier, 1998), which are certain areas of the human intellect, through which the structuring of disparate but related elements, roles, propositions and relationships takes place. Mental spaces, as it were, replace certain existing worlds and situations, since “taken from specific linear texts, they reflect human understanding of hypothetical and fictional situations and help to penetrate deeper into the manifestations of discourse” (Manerko, 2002, 21), reflecting the human ability to “create a model world that inherits information from the real world or from another model of the world” (Dinsmore, 1996, 400). Mental spaces are characterized by the following features: firstly, they include certain mental entities (different types of mental representations), secondly, they can be structured by special cognitive models, and thirdly, mental spaces can enter into a relationship of conceptual integration. , called Fauconnier conceptual blending (mixing).

An elementary unit of mental resources, many scientists, following E.S. Kubryakova, consider the concept: "The concept is ... an operational meaningful unit of memory, mental lexicon, conceptual system and language of the brain" (Kubryakova, KSKT, 90), and this unit acts as a completely independent and clearly distinguished entity. Concepts are formed in the course of conceptual classification, or conceptualization.

The use of the term "concept" itself is not the same in different languages. A detailed study of the spheres of use of this term was undertaken by V.Z. Demyankov, who considers the use of the term "concept" in Russian, Latin, French, Spanish, Italian, English and German based on a large corpus of texts of various genres (Demyankov, 2001).

It seems to us necessary to consider the relationship between the terms "concept", "meaning" and "notion" and their differentiation.

R.I. Pavilenis identifies the concepts of "concept" and "meaning": "The process of human cognition ... is the process of forming meanings, or concepts, about objects of knowledge as a process of constructing information about them. This information is about the current or possible state of things in the world ... and is what we call "meaning" or "concept" (Pavilenis, 1983, 101-102).The totality of such formations is a "conceptual system built from interrelated concepts - meanings" (Pavilenis, 1986, 241).

According to Yu. Stepanov in his work "Constants: Dictionary of Russian Culture" - the concept and concept are terms of different sciences; the term "concept" is used mainly in logic and philosophy, and the term "concept", which was previously a term exclusively for mathematical logic, has also become entrenched in cultural studies. It is the main term of the Dictionary:

"A concept is, as it were, a clot of culture in the mind of a person; that in the form of which culture enters the mental world of a person" (Stepanov, 2001, 43).

So, we talk about concepts first of all when we need to characterize the units of thought processes. Accordingly, the process of structuring consciousness, as a result of which concepts arise, is called, following foreign cognitivists, conceptualization. The purpose of the conceptualization process is to highlight the minimum units of human experience in their meaningful representation.

In their totality, concepts are combined into a conceptual system (in other terminology - a conceptual picture of the world, a model of the world, an image of the world), since "general knowledge is not amorphous, it is organized into conceptual systems (van Dijk, 1989, 16). The most complete description of conceptual systems is given in the works of R.I. Pavilenis, in which the conceptual system is interpreted as a system of opinions and knowledge about the world, reflecting the cognitive experience of a person at the pre-linguistic and linguistic levels and irreducible to any kind of linguistic entity (Pavilenis, 1983). "assimilation new information about the world is carried out by the individual on the basis of that which he already has at his disposal. The system of information about the world thus formed is the conceptual system constructed by him as a system of certain ideas of a person about the world" (Pavilenis, 1983, 101).

Linguistics establishes a connection between the picture of the world and language, studies ways of fixing the mental content by means of language, in other words, the linguistic picture of the world, which is formed on the basis of a conceptual picture of the world. "The picture of the world as a subjective image of objective reality, without ceasing to be an image of reality, is objectified in sign forms" (Serebrennikov et al. kol., 1988, 21).

The linguistic picture of the world is a set of ideas about the world, historically formed in the ordinary consciousness of a given linguistic community and reflected in the language, a certain way of conceptualizing reality. The concept of the linguistic picture of the world goes back to the ideas of W. von Humboldt about the internal form of language, on the one hand, and to the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis of linguistic relativity, on the other. V. von Humboldt recognized that language and thinking are inextricably linked, believing that each person has a subjective image of a certain object that does not completely coincide with the image of the same object in another person, and this representation can be objectified only by paving "one's own way through the mouth into the outer world" (Humboldt, 1985, 168). The word, therefore, bears the burden of subjective ideas, since their carriers have a certain national character and consciousness.

The studies of E. Sapir and B. Whorf revealed the specifics of the categorization of the world among different peoples and served as the beginning of the study of cognitive categories, since the researchers came to the conclusion that each language structures the world in a special way for its speakers (Sapir, 1993). “We single out certain categories and types in the world of phenomena not at all because they are self-evident; on the contrary, the world appears before us as a kaleidoscopic stream of impressions, which must be organized by our consciousness, and this means mainly by the language system stored in our consciousness” (Whorf, 1960, 174).

Theory of the language picture of the world in modern science is in the stage of reflection and active development. Two main directions in the study of this issue can be distinguished: philosophical, coming from Hegel (Brutyan, 1976; Pavilenis, 1983) and linguistic (Karaulov 1976, 1981; Kolshansky, 1990, Postovalova, 1988, etc.).

Modern ideas about the linguistic picture of the world are as follows: each natural language reflects a certain way of perceiving and conceptualizing the world. The meanings expressed in it add up to a certain unified system of views, a kind of collective philosophy. The way of conceptualizing reality inherent in a given language is partly universal, partly nationally specific, so that speakers different languages can see the world in different ways, which is due to the culture of the speaking community, its mentality, vision of the world, and language. (Apresyan, 1995) or as "linguistic images of real objects and relations, peripheral areas of verbal representations, which become a source of additional information about the reality around us. Moreover, they often produce persistent deposits in the mind of the cognizing subject due to the figurative nature of their information" (Vereshchagin, Kostomarov, 1983, 168).

In accordance with this, studies of the linguistic picture of the world are aimed primarily at the study of individual linguo-specific concepts that are “key” for a given culture and for which there is no translation equivalent (as, for example, for German words riicksichtvoll, die Rucksichtslosigkeit etc., or for Russian words daring, outburst etc).

Speaking about the relationship between cultural (or conceptual, conceptual) and linguistic pictures of the world, some researchers believe that the conceptual and linguistic pictures of the world correlate with each other as a whole with a part, since the linguistic picture is poorer than the cultural picture due to the fact that, along with with language, and other types of mental activity (Serebrennikov et al. coll., 1988, 107). Others supplement this statement, considering the fact of interpenetration, interconnection and interaction to be important. (Ter-Minasova, 2000). Being a way of storing linguistic knowledge and knowledge about the world, the linguistic picture of the world is not independent, it is inseparable from the conceptual picture of the world. According to G.V. Kolshansky, "language is a form of mastery of the world, but not a form of a special world. That is why it is impossible to speak separately about linguistic consciousness, separately about the linguistic third world and separately about the linguistic picture of the world"

(Kolshansky, 1990, 16). Undoubtedly, the national cultural picture of the world is primary in relation to the linguistic one. It is fuller, richer and deeper than the corresponding language. However, it is the language that realizes, verbalizes the national cultural picture of the world, stores it and passes it on from generation to generation. “The vocabulary of a particular language includes, in general, along with the totality of linguistic signs, also the totality of conceptual mental means that the language community has; and as each native speaker learns this vocabulary, all members of the language community master these mental means; In this sense, it can be said that the possibility mother tongue consists in the fact that he contains in his concepts a certain picture of the world and conveys it to all members of the language community,” L. Weisgerber wrote back in 1930 (quoted from Radchenko, 2005, 250). Therefore, the linguistic picture of the world can be represented as a set of representatives of background knowledge.

In this regard, the study of the linguistic picture of the world, which is “an intermediary between the psychological and cognitive mechanisms of human consciousness and the surrounding world” (Manerko, 2002, 19), allows researchers to penetrate deeper into the structure and elements of mental processes. At the same time, interactive processes of interaction between two semiotic systems (language and culture) are studied from the standpoint of the cultural and linguistic competence of the speaker/listener. The explication of the cognitive procedures carried out by the subject when interpreting the culturally significant reference of linguistic signs is carried out on the material of the living functioning of the language in discourses of various types in order to study cultural self-awareness.

So, operating with concepts in cognitive and mental activity, a person distributes incoming information into classes in accordance with certain characteristics, while resorting to language: "All language units serve either to express information or divide the information flow. By doing this, they serve as thought processes in the head of an individual, and contribute to the reflection of the experience of mankind as a whole, fixing the results of perception and cognition of reality" (Kubryakova, 2004, 235).

Cognitive articulation of reality is associated with categorization processes, the essence of which is the division of the entire ontological space into different categorical areas. With the help of categories, a person "selects impulses and impressions coming from the external world, and transforms them into data of his inner experience ... These categories are imprinted in the language, as well as in other sign systems.... and it is impossible to think about the world without using these categories. (Gurevich, 1984, 31).

The concept of category and categorization is one of the most fundamental concepts of human activity and one of the key concepts of the cognitive approach. The ability to classify objects and phenomena, distributing them into different classes, types, categories and categories indicates that in the process of perceiving the world, a person constantly draws conclusions about the identity of some objects to others or about their difference. Categorization is the main way of ordering the world, systematization of objects and phenomena. Unlike the process of conceptualization, the process of categorization is aimed at grouping similar units into larger groups.

Categorization in the narrow sense - bringing a phenomenon, object, process, etc. under a certain heading of experience - a category - and recognizing it as a member of this category. In more broad sense categorization - the process of formation and selection of the categories themselves, the division of the external and internal world of a person in accordance with the essential characteristics of his functioning and being, the ordered presentation of various phenomena by reducing them to a smaller number of categories or associations, as well as the result of classification (taxonomic) activity. Thus, categorization is one of the key concepts in the description cognitive activity of a person, associated with almost all cognitive abilities and systems in his cognitive apparatus, as well as with operations performed in the processes of thinking - comparison, identification, establishment of similarity and similarity.

Categorization as a type of classification activity has attracted the attention of scientists since ancient times, starting from antiquity. The first presentation of the categorical spectrum was undertaken by Aristotle in his work "Categories" (Aristotle, 1975; 1978), who summarized the attempts of previous philosophers and compiled a table of categories, thus highlighting the categories of essence, quantity, quality, relationship, place, time, position , states, actions, suffering. Aristotle's table had a decisive influence on the development of the doctrine of categories up to modern times.

The term "conceptual categories" in linguistics was introduced into scientific use by O. Jespersen. O. Jespersen admits that "along with syntactic categories that depend on the structure of each language, there are also extralinguistic categories that do not depend on more or less random facts existing languages. These categories are universal in that they apply to all languages, although they are rarely expressed in those languages ​​in a clear and unambiguous way" (Jespersen, 1958, 57).

The existence of a "universal component" of the language from the proper linguistic positions was substantiated by W. von Humboldt: "Universal categories are for the most part mental forms of logical origin. They form a system that is the general basis of the language, but is not directly included in the structure of the language" (cit. according to Katsnelson, 1986, 12).

G. Paul in his work "Principles of the History of Language" dwells in sufficient detail on such categories, calling them "psychological categories". He believes that every grammatical category arises on the basis of psychological, and the first is nothing more than an external expression of the second. As soon as the effectiveness of the psychological category begins to be revealed in linguistic means, this category becomes grammatical (Paul, 1960). Note that this provision obviously echoes Humboldt's idea of ​​"transforming" the universal categories he considers into specific grammatical categories.

In Russian linguistics, I.I. Meshchaninov. His typological studies led him to the conclusion that “the same concept can be conveyed by various means, which is most clearly seen in comparative comparisons of materials from different systems of languages. In some languages ​​it is expressed in the semantic grouping of words, in others the same norms are reflected in the grammatical construction of words and phrases in a sentence, etc.” (Meshchaninov, 1981, 58).

The identification of this universal mental substrate became in the works of I.I. Meshchaninov with problems related to the analysis of conceptual categories (Meshchaninov, 1958): “Any concept that exists in the human mind can be conveyed by means of language. It can be expressed descriptively, it can be conveyed by the semantics of a single word, it can form a certain system in its linguistic transmission. In the latter case, a conceptual category appears... Thus, not every concept transmitted by the language is a conceptual category. It becomes such a concept that appears in the linguistic system and receives a certain construction in it. The latter finds its expression in a certain lexical, morphological or syntactic system” (Meshchaninov, 1981, 60).

These studies were further developed in the works of A.V. Bondarko (Bondarko, 1971, 1978, 1984, TFG 1996; 1998). Speaking not about conceptual, but about semantic categories A.V. Bondarko points out that both of them develop based on certain linguistic means of their expression: "there is a direction from the original supralinguistic content system to its concrete language and speech representation" (Bondarko, 1998, 22).

A significant contribution to the study of the conceptual sphere of thinking in its relation to language was made by WL Chaif. He believes that conceptual structure and surface structure are two different things: if the surface structure is represented by the material means of language and given to us in sensory perception, then concepts are located deep within the human nervous system (Chafe, 1975).

Considering the process of categorization as one of the main processes of human cognitive activity, linguistics present stage raises the question of “on the basis of which an ordinary person classifies things and how he reduces the diversity of his sensations and the objective diversity of the forms of matter and the forms of its movement into certain headings” (Kubryakova, KSKT, p. 46-47), that is, on the basis of what a person makes his judgments about the similarity or difference and spreads the perceived into certain groups. Over the past decade, a huge number of publications have appeared, covering the problems of categories and categorization from a variety of points of view. Thus, dealing with the issue of harmony between the various categories of a particular language, as well as the reasons for the absence of categories in one language that play a central role in another, Durst-Andersen puts forward the hypothesis that "not all languages ​​are grammaticalized on the same basis" (Durst-Andersen, 1995, 31).

The category of quality, covering all spheres of human life - production, spiritual, mental, sphere interpersonal relationships and others, is one of the fundamental ontological categories. It is a category constituting "the initial stage of cognition of things and the formation of the world, a direct description of the objects of being" (FES 1983, 253), thanks to which it becomes possible to extract information about the picture of the world, value orientations and the inner world of communicants.

The category of quality “acts as a reflection and designation of the attributes of matter, that is, the universal properties that are inherent in a material object and are associated with the existence of an object. It really exists, develops and contains its own essence, regularity and results of its own action and development in itself (Ilyin, 1972, 3). Being ontological in its basis, the category of quality exists independently of human consciousness. But being, mediated by the socio-historical activity of people, is reflected in our consciousness and finds its expression in thinking and language, being divided into mental and linguistic content.

Mental content refers to conceptual categories according to Jespersen, that is, universal categories (Jespersen, 1958). Speaking about the linguistic content of a category, we mean the meanings of linguistic units. So, the actual conceptual quality is designated as a category of quality, and to designate the linguistic content of the ontological category of quality, some researchers propose to use the term attribution (Glushak, 1999, 34). Her research was carried out throughout the history of linguistics. Different scientific paradigms considered attributes in accordance with their goals and objectives, which made it possible to identify the various essences of this phenomenon. The most common interpretation of an attribute as a sign of an object, that is, the characterization of a substance in the totality of its properties, actions, states, etc. Such an understanding of attributes classifies not only nouns, but also adjectives, adverbs, verbs and other parts of speech into the category of a characterized substance (Katsnelson, 1972).

The traditional interpretation of the attribute as a member of the sentence, which refers only to the dominant name and forms a substantive combination with it (Admoni, 1986, 222-223), for many years limited his study to the adjective within the substantive group. Particularly indicative in this regard are the views of linguists of the early 20th century, as well as structuralists and generativists, on the essence of an attribute. The former understood by attribute only a definition to a noun (Blatz, 1900; Sutterlin, 1900), etc., or, more broadly, as a definition to any word in a sentence, excluding the verb, since such a definition would apply to the entire sentence and would be essentially a circumstance (Becker, 1956; Jung, 1953). The basis for classifying attributes as attribute words was the ability of the latter to predicate, and from a functional point of view, according to generativists, it is hardly possible to draw a sharp distinction between an attribute and a predicate (Chafe 1975; Hartung, 1968; Helbig, 1969; Schmidt 1962, 1970). Proponents of generative linguistics believed that the attribute is a superficial characteristic for certain members of the sentence, to which various deeper relationships and semantic interpretations should be attributed. The value of the aforementioned studies undoubtedly lies in the fact that they have discovered the enormous semantic potential of attributes, and their ability to express a wide variety of meanings.

Lexico-grammatical means of expressing the category of quality have been studied in detail in linguistic works within the framework of the structural and functional-semantic approach (Bondarko, 1996; Vorotnikov, 1999; Wolf, 1985; Katsnelson, 1972; Nikolaeva, 1983; Sergeeva, 1980), including material of the German language (Admoni, 1986; Burdina, 1987; Gulyga, Shendels, 1969; Moskalskaya, 1983). So, Z.G. Burdina, exploring indecomposable structures with a qualitative component of meaning, makes the following very important conclusion - in attributive constructions, the integration of their components is traced, which ensures the emergence of a qualitative meaning. Grammatical sign quality in the noun group turns out to be embedded in the formal structure of the structure itself - both the nominal and the attributive component, denoting size, color, shape (Burdina 1987, 55).

The perception and conceptualization of any material entity, and especially the human person, is impossible without defining its qualities as a necessary condition for existence. Since quality is a direct determinant of matter or human essence and expresses “its essential certainty, inseparable from the being of an object, due to which it is precisely this and not another object (FES 1985, 252), then the perception and conceptualization of any human personality, any person, is impossible without defining its qualitative parameters as a necessary condition for existence.

The new paradigm of linguistic knowledge allows us to consider the category of quality as a certain set of concepts of qualities, determined by the mentality and value system of society.

Further analysis is aimed at comparing and generalizing the described data in order to "build" them into a certain concept, a "mini-theory" of the described events.

Primary data classification Classification, or nomination, of data- this is a combination of the content of textual information into a certain generalized category - into one class. It is necessary to compare different information (evidence) received from one respondent within one "case" or to compare this "case" with similar ones. So, statements about parents, relatives can be combined into the class "attitude to the family."

Some data may be classified ("nominated") immediately. For example, in our excerpt from the biography of the "Arbat resident" P., we record his gender and place of birth. Other information requires understanding from the point of view of scientific concepts. For example, the passage cited can be broadly classified under the concept of "identity"; or more specifically in terms of "identification with a small homeland"; "identification with the liberal intelligentsia". Such a primary nomination depends on the objectives of the study and requires further clarification (narrowing or, conversely, expanding) after comparison with other episodes of the same biography or interviews with other people. The concept of "identity" is appropriate if we are going to interpret the problem in theories of personal "we-identification". However, the same fragment can be classified in terms of "primary cultural environment", or rather, "cultural environment of the Arbat", if the problem of changes in cultural orientations throughout a person's life path is analyzed.

Here it is appropriate to recall once again that the direction of analysis depends not only on the content of the text, but, of course, also on the research interest sociologist, his attitude problematizing events. In fact, in the excerpt from the interview with P., there are both problems of social identification, and the problem of the life development of the cultural environment as they grow older and socio-historical changes in this environment itself. The sociologist is more likely to choose to conceptualize in terms of cultural milieu, while the social psychologist is likely to prefer to classify relevant narrative fragments in terms of personal identification.

The logic of the analysis may lead to the need to introduce additional nominations that were not previously provided or have no basis in this passage. Generalization of nominations is a method by which private data are combined into a certain set of similar phenomena that can be expressed in a system of sociological concepts.

Theoretical understanding of data- a very delicate and time-consuming analytical work. In the process of classification, one has to repeatedly refer to the primary text again and again in order to more accurately formulate a sociological concept that is most suitable for all fragments, passages, and all analyzed texts. Such analytical-intuitive activity of the researcher requires imagination, understanding of the text and the social reality behind it, but above all, sufficient sociological culture. eighteen

18 The famous logician Mario Bunge wrote that intuition is "trash in the attic of our memory" from which, if necessary, we extract the necessary objects.

There is an interaction between the problem-theoretical "field" of the researcher and a fragment of social reality, represented by the text, during which different categories are "tried on" and the final choice of the paradigm most suitable for this study takes place. The solution does not have to be mechanically neutral; it comes from certain research goals - what will I achieve as a result of using certain classification concepts?

Consider an example of the classification of statements about the role and authority of the father in the family from the study by E. Meshcherkina [172. p. 312]; "... Conflicts with my stepfather? No, there were, of course, sometimes. But, by the way, he never liked to teach me anything or read morality there. Mostly my mother, of course. She even forced him to whip me with a strap, but "It was rare. I don't get angry at them, And he thought that I should understand everything myself, for which I am grateful to him ... He did not impose his opinion on me, but when I had already grown up, I was already stronger ... Besides, I was then two years old, so 9-10 grade, I was engaged in karate, I could do something. I even remember a couple of times - I beat him. And since then he hasn’t bothered me at all. Well, so, he’ll yell ... "

Classification:

* relationship with father (use of force in education, "hard" practice)

* the role of the stepfather and mother in education (the peripheral role of the stepfather, the active role of the mother)

* change in family relations as the son grows up (playing sports, realizing aggressiveness, dominance)

* son's response to his stepfather (reciprocal use of force in a dispute with him)

Later, according to the classifications put forward here, it is possible to compare the system of relations between father and son with other cases where similar information is presented. It is possible that some of the categories will be clarified, others enlarged, additional categories will appear, but it is the primary classifications of this interview that become the basis for a comparative analysis of the father-son relationship in other texts.

Classifications should not overlap, they resemble an unordered nominal scale in a quantitative approach: the described fragments are either "included" or "excluded" in this nomination. Subsequently, causal links (dependency links) between the classified fragments can be revealed only if this requirement is met.

Classification rules:

Classification is the first step in conceptual analysis.

It provides for a break in the continuous text and its new construction, now on the basis of generalized concepts.

The classification is based on the combination of common sense with the analytical intuition of the researcher.

The main purpose of classification is the comparison of fragmentary data, their generalization.

Multiple revision of the selected categories contributes to a more adequate nomination.

The final conceptualization should correspond to the problematic goals of the study.

It is the classification that underlies the identification of dependencies. Now that everyday concepts are classified, we can identify patterns, variations and deviations in data, discover common meanings and communication patterns within the same community.


Similar information.